An official website of the United States government

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

PLOS ONE logo

Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators

Charlotte r pennington, andrew r levy, derek t larkin.

  • Author information
  • Article notes
  • Copyright and License information

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Analyzed the data: CRP DH. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CRP DH ARL DTL. Wrote the paper: CRP. Developed the review design and protocol: CRP DH AL DL. Reviewed the manuscript: DH AL DL. Cross-checked articles in systematic review: CRP DH.

* E-mail: [email protected]

Received 2015 Jun 23; Accepted 2015 Dec 17; Collection date 2016.

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

This systematic literature review appraises critically the mediating variables of stereotype threat. A bibliographic search was conducted across electronic databases between 1995 and 2015. The search identified 45 experiments from 38 articles and 17 unique proposed mediators that were categorized into affective/subjective ( n = 6), cognitive ( n = 7) and motivational mechanisms ( n = 4). Empirical support was accrued for mediators such as anxiety, negative thinking, and mind-wandering, which are suggested to co-opt working memory resources under stereotype threat. Other research points to the assertion that stereotype threatened individuals may be motivated to disconfirm negative stereotypes, which can have a paradoxical effect of hampering performance. However, stereotype threat appears to affect diverse social groups in different ways, with no one mediator providing unequivocal empirical support. Underpinned by the multi-threat framework, the discussion postulates that different forms of stereotype threat may be mediated by distinct mechanisms.

Introduction

The present review examines the mediators of stereotype threat that have been proposed over the past two decades. It appraises critically the underlying mechanisms of stereotype threat as a function of the type of threat primed, the population studied, and the measures utilized to examine mediation and performance outcomes. Here, we propose that one reason that has precluded studies from finding firm evidence of mediation is the appreciation of distinct forms of stereotype threat.

Stereotype Threat: An Overview

Over the past two decades, stereotype threat has become one of the most widely researched topics in social psychology [ 1 , 2 ]. Reaching its 20 th anniversary, Steele and Aronson’s [ 3 ] original article has gathered approximately 5,000 citations and has been referred to as a 'modern classic' [ 4 , 5 , 6 ]. In stark contrast to theories of genetic intelligence [ 7 , 8 ] (and see [ 9 ] for debate), the theory of stereotype threat posits that stigmatized group members may underperform on diagnostic tests of ability through concerns about confirming a negative societal stereotype as self-characteristic [ 3 ]. Steele and Aronson [ 3 ] demonstrated that African American participants underperformed on a verbal reasoning test when it was presented as a diagnostic indicator of intellectual ability. Conversely, when the same test was presented as non-diagnostic of ability, they performed equivalently to their Caucasian peers. This seminal research indicates that the mere salience of negative societal stereotypes, which may magnify over time, can impede performance. The theory of stereotype threat therefore offers a situational explanation for the ongoing and intractable debate regarding the source of group differences in academic aptitude [ 1 ].

Stereotype threat has been used primarily to explain gaps in intellectual and quantitative test scores between African and European Americans [ 3 , 10 ] and women and men respectively [ 11 ]. However, it is important to acknowledge that many factors shape academic performance, and stereotype threat is unlikely to be the sole explanation for academic achievement gaps [ 12 ]. This is supported by research which has shown “pure” stereotype threat effects on a task in which a gender-achievement gap has not been previously documented [ 13 ], thus suggesting that performance decrements can be elicited simply by reference to a negative stereotype. Furthermore, stereotype threat effects may not be limited to social groups who routinely face stigmatizing attitudes. Rather, it can befall anyone who is a member of a group to which a negative stereotype applies [ 3 ]. For example, research indicates that Caucasian men, a group that have a relatively positive social status, underperform when they believe that their mathematical performance will be compared to that of Asian men [ 14 ]. White men also appear to perform worse than black men when motor tasks are related to 'natural athletic ability' [ 15 , 16 ]. From a theoretical standpoint, stereotype threat exposes how group stereotypes may shape the behavior of individuals in a way that endangers their performance and further reinforces the stereotype [ 10 ].

Over 300 experiments have illustrated the deleterious and extensive effects that stereotype threat can inflict on many different populations [ 17 ]. The possibility of confirming a negative stereotype about one’s group is found to contribute to underperformance on a range of diverse tasks including intelligence [ 3 , 13 ], memory [ 18 , 19 ], mental rotation [ 20 – 23 ], and math tests [ 11 , 24 , 25 ], golf putting [ 26 ], driving [ 27 , 28 ] and childcare skills [ 29 ]. Given the generality of these findings, researchers have turned their efforts to elucidating the underlying mechanisms of this situational phenomenon.

Susceptibility to Stereotype Threat

Research has identified numerous moderators that make tasks more likely to elicit stereotype threat, and individuals more prone to experience it [ 30 , 31 ]. From a methodological perspective, stereotype threat effects tend to emerge on tasks of high difficulty and demand [ 32 , 33 ], however, the extent to which a task is perceived as demanding may be moderated by individual differences in working memory [ 34 ]. Additionally, stereotype threat may be more likely to occur when individuals are conscious of the stigma ascribed to their social group [ 32 , 35 ], believe the stereotypes about their group to be true [ 36 , 37 ], for those with low self-esteem [ 38 ], and an internal locus of control [ 39 ]. Research also indicates that individuals are more susceptible to stereotype threat when they identify strongly with their social group [ 40 , 41 , 42 ] and value the domain [ 10 , 13 , 15 , 33 , 43 ]. However, other research suggests that domain identification is not a prerequisite of stereotype threat effects [ 44 ] and may act as a strategy to overcome harmful academic consequences [ 45 , 46 ].

Mediators of Stereotype Threat

There has also been an exPLOSion of research into the psychological mediators of stereotype threat (c.f. [ 2 , 47 ] for reviews). In their comprehensive review, Schmader et al. [ 2 ] proposed an integrated process model, suggesting that stereotype threat heightens physiological stress responses and influences monitoring and suppression processes to deplete working memory efficiency. This provides an important contribution to the literature, signaling that multiple affective, cognitive and motivational processes may underpin the effects of stereotype threat on performance. However, the extent to which each of these variables has garnered empirical support remains unclear. Furthermore, prior research has overlooked the existence of distinct stereotype threats in the elucidation of mediating variables. Through the lens of the multi-threat framework [ 31 ], the current review distinguishes between different stereotype threat primes, which target either the self or the social group to assess the evidence base with regards to the existence of multiple stereotype threats that may be accounted for by distinct mechanisms.

A Multi-threat Approach to Mediation

Stereotype threat is typically viewed as a form of social identity threat: A situational predicament occurring when individuals perceive their social group to be devalued by others [ 48 , 49 , 50 ]. However, this notion overlooks how individuals may self-stigmatize and evaluate themselves [ 51 , 52 , 53 ] and the conflict people may experience between their personal and social identities [ 54 ]. More recently, researchers have distinguished between the role of the self and the social group in performance-evaluative situations [ 31 ]. The multi-threat framework [ 31 ] identifies six qualitatively distinct stereotype threats that manifest through the intersection of two dimensions: The target of the threat (i.e., is the stereotype applicable to one’s personal or social identity?) and the source of threat (i.e., who will judge performance; the in-group or the out-group?). Focusing on the target of the stereotype, individuals who experience a group-as-target threat may perceive that underperformance will confirm a negative societal stereotype regarding the abilities of their social group. Conversely, individuals who experience a self-as-target threat may perceive that stereotype-consistent performance will be viewed as self-characteristic [ 31 , 55 ]. Individuals may therefore experience either a self or group-based threat dependent on situational cues in the environment that heighten the contingency of a stereotyped identity [ 2 ].

Researchers also theorize that members of diverse stigmatized groups may experience different forms of stereotype threat [ 31 , 56 ], and that these distinct experiences may be mediated by somewhat different processes [ 31 , 57 ]. Indeed, there is some indirect empirical evidence to suggest that this may be the case. For example, Pavlova and colleagues [ 13 ] found that an implicit stereotype threat prime hampered women’s performance on a social cognition task. Conversely, men’s performance suffered when they were primed with an explicit gender-related stereotype. Moreover, Stone and McWhinnie [ 58 ] suggest that subtle stereotype threat cues (i.e., the gender of the experimenter) may evoke a tendency to actively monitor performance and avoid mistakes, whereas blatant stereotype threat cues (i.e., stereotype prime) create distractions that deplete working memory resources. Whilst different stereotype threat cues may simultaneously exert negative effects on performance, it is plausible that they are induced by independent mechanisms [ 58 ]. Nonetheless, insufficient evidence has prevented the multi-threat framework [ 31 ] to be evaluated empirically to date. It therefore remains to be assessed whether the same mechanisms are responsible for the effects of distinct stereotype threats on different populations and performance measures.

The current article offers the first systematic literature review aiming to: 1), identify and examine critically the proposed mediators of stereotype threat; 2), explore whether the effects of self-as-target or group-as-target stereotype threat on performance are the result of qualitatively distinct mediating mechanisms; and 3), evaluate whether different mediators govern different stereotyped populations.

Literature Search

A bibliographic search of electronic databases, such as PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, Web of Knowledge, PubMed, Science Direct and Google Scholar was conducted between the cut-off dates of 1995 (the publication year of Steele & Aronson’s seminal article) and December 2015. A search string was developed by specifying the main terms of the phenomenon under investigation. Here, the combined key words of stereotype and threat were utilized as overarching search parameters and directly paired with either one of the following terms; mediator , mediating , mediate(s) , predictor , predicts , relationship or mechanism(s) . Additional references were retrieved by reviewing the reference lists of relevant journal articles. To control for potential publication bias [ 59 , 60 , 61 ], the lead author also enquired about any ‘in press’ articles by sending out a call for papers through the European Association for Social Psychology. The second author conducted a comparable search using the same criteria to ensure that no studies were overlooked in the original search. Identification of relevant articles and data extraction were conducted in line with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Statement (PRISMA; See S1 Table ) [ 62 ]. A literature search was conducted separately in each database and the records were exported to citation software, after which duplicates were removed. Relevant articles were screened by examining the title and abstract in line with the eligibility criteria. The remaining articles were assessed for eligibility by performing a full text review [ 63 , 64 ].

Eligibility Criteria

Studies were selected based on the following criteria: 1), researchers utilized a stereotype threat manipulation; 2), a direct mediation analysis was conducted between stereotype threat and performance; 3), researchers found evidence of moderated-mediation, and 4), the full text was available in English. Articles were excluded on the following basis: 1), performance was not the dependent variable, 2), investigations of “stereotype lift”; 3), doctorate, dissertation and review articles (to avoid duplication of included articles); and 4), moderating variables. Articles that did not find any significant results in relation to stereotype threat effects were also excluded in order to capture reliable evidence of mediation [ 65 ]. See Table 1 for details of excluded articles.

Table 1. Number of articles excluded in full text review, with reasons.

Distinguishing different stereotype threats.

The current review distinguished between different experiences of stereotype threat by examining each stereotype threat manipulation. Self-as-target threats were categorized on the basis that participants focused on the test as a measure of personal ability whereas group-as-target threats were classified on the basis that participants perceived performance to be diagnostic of their group’s ability [ 31 ].

A total of 45 studies in 38 articles were qualitatively synthesized, uncovering a total of 17 distinct proposed mediators. See Fig 1 for process of article inclusion (full details of article exclusion can be viewed in S1 Supporting Information ). These mediators were categorized into affective/subjective ( n = 6), cognitive ( n = 7) or motivational mechanisms ( n = 4). Effect sizes for mediational findings are described typically through informal descriptors, such as complete , perfect , or partial [ 66 ]. With this in mind, the current findings are reported in terms of complete or partial mediation. Complete mediation indicates that the relationship between stereotype threat ( X ) and performance ( Y ) completely disappears when a mediator ( M ) is added as a predictor variable [ 66 ]. Partial mediation refers to instances in which a significant direct effect remains between stereotype threat and performance when controlling for the mediator, suggesting that additional variables may further explain this relationship [ 67 ]. Instances of moderated mediation are also reported, which occurs when the strength of mediation is contingent on the level of a moderating variable [ 68 ]. The majority of included research utilized a group-as-target prime ( n = 36, 80%) compared to a self-as-target prime ( n = 6; 13.33%). Three studies (6.66%) were uncategorized as they employed subtle stereotype threat primes, for example, manipulating the group composition of the testing environment.

Fig 1. Process of article inclusion (following PRISMA).

Fig 1

Affective/Subjective Mechanisms

Researchers have conceptualized stereotype threat frequently as a fear, apprehension or anxiety of confirming a negative stereotype about one’s group [ 3 , 69 , 70 ]. Accordingly, many affective and subjective variables such as anxiety, individuation tendencies, evaluation apprehension, performance expectations, explicit stereotype endorsement and self-efficacy have been proposed to account for the stereotype threat-performance relationship.

Steele and Aronson’s [ 3 ] original study did not find self-reported anxiety to be a significant mediator of the effects of a self-relevant stereotype on African American’s intellectual performance. Extending this work, Spencer et al. (Experiment 3, [ 11 ]) found that anxiety was not predictive of the effects that a negative group stereotype had on women’s mathematical achievement, with further research confirming this [ 14 , 44 , 71 ]. Additional studies have indicated that self-reported anxiety does not influence the impact of self-as-target stereotype elicitation on African American’s cognitive ability [ 72 ], white students’ athletic skills [ 15 ], and group-as-target stereotype threat on older adults’ memory recall [ 18 , 32 ].

Research also suggests that anxiety may account for one of multiple mediators in the stereotype threat-performance relationship. In a field study, Chung and colleagues [ 73 ] found that self-reported state anxiety and specific self-efficacy sequentially mediated the influence of stereotype threat on African American’s promotional exam performance. This finding is supported by Mrazek et al. [ 74 ] who found that anxiety and mind-wandering sequentially mediated the effects of stereotype threat on women’s mathematical ability. Laurin [ 75 ] also found that self-reported somatic anxiety partially mediated the effects of group-as-target stereotype threat on women’s motor performance. Nevertheless, it is viable to question whether this finding is comparable to other studies as stereotype threat had a facilitating effect on performance.

The mixed results regarding anxiety as a potential mediator of performance outcomes may be indicative of various boundary conditions that enhance stereotype threat susceptibility. Consistent with this claim, Gerstenberg, Imhoff and Schmitt (Experiment 3 [ 76 ]) found that women who reported a fragile math self-concept solved fewer math problems under group-as-target stereotype threat and this susceptibility was mediated by increased anxiety. This moderated-mediation suggests that women with a low academic self-concept may be more vulnerable to stereotype threat, with anxiety underpinning its effect on mathematical performance.

Given that anxiety may be relatively difficult to detect via self-report measures [ 3 , 29 ], researchers have utilized indirect measures. For instance, Bosson et al. [ 29 ] found that physiological anxiety mediated the effects of stereotype threat on homosexual males’ performance on an interpersonal task. Nevertheless, this effect has not been replicated for the effects of group-as-target stereotype threat on older adults’ memory recall [ 32 ] and self-as-target threat on children’s writing ability [ 77 ].

Individuation tendencies

Steele and Aronson [ 3 ] proposed that stereotype threat might occur when individuals perceive a negative societal stereotype to be a true representation of personal ability. Based on this, Keller and Sekaquaptewa [ 78 ] examined whether gender-related threats (i.e., group-as-target threat) influenced women to individuate their personal identity (the self) from their social identity (female). Results revealed that participants underperformed on a spatial ability test when they perceived that they were a single in-group representative (female) in a group of males. Moreover, stereotype threat was partially mediated by ‘individuation tendencies’ in that gender-based threats influenced women to disassociate their self from the group to lessen the applicability of the stereotype. The authors suggest that this increased level of self-focused attention under solo status conditions is likely related to increased levels of anxiety.

Evaluation apprehension

Steele and Aronson [ 3 ] also suggested that individuals might apprehend that they will confirm a negative stereotype in the eyes of out-group members. Despite this, Mayer and Hanges [ 72 ] found that evaluation apprehension did not mediate the effects of a self-as-target stereotype threat on African American’s cognitive ability. Additional studies also indicate that evaluation apprehension does not mediate the effects of group-as-target stereotype threat on women’s mathematical performance [ 11 , 79 ].

Performance expectations

Under stereotype threat, individuals may evaluate the subjective likelihood of success depending on their personal resources. As these personal resources are typically anchored to group-level expectations, in-group threatening information (i.e., women are poor at math) may reduce personal expectancies to achieve and diminish performance [ 80 ]. Testing this prediction, Cadinu et al. (Experiment 1 [ 80 ]) found that women solved fewer math problems when they were primed with a negative group-based stereotype relative to those who received a positive or no stereotype. Furthermore, performance expectancies partially mediated the effect of group-as-target threat on math performance, revealing that negative information was associated with lower expectancies. A second experiment indicated further that performance expectancies partially mediated the effects of group-as-target threat on Black participants’ verbal ability. Research by Rosenthal, Crisp and Mein-Woei (Experiment 2 [ 81 ]) also found that performance expectancies partially mediated the effects of self-based stereotypes on women’s mathematical performance. However, rather than decreasing performance expectancies, women under stereotype threat reported higher predictions for performance relative to a control condition.

Research has extended this work to examine the role of performance expectancies in diverse stigmatized populations. For example, Hess et al. [ 32 ] found evidence of moderated-mediation for the effects of a group-as-target stereotype threat on older adults’ memory recall. Here, the degree to which performance expectancies mediated stereotype threat effects was moderated by participants’ education. That is, elderly individuals with higher levels of education showed greater susceptibility to stereotype threat. These findings add weight to the assertion that lowered performance expectations may account for the effects of stereotype threat on performance, especially among individuals who identify strongly with the ability domain. Conversely, Appel et al. [ 43 ] found that performance expectancies do not mediate the effects of group-based stereotype threat among highly identified women in the domains of science, technology, engineering and mathematics.

Further research suggests that stereotype threat can be activated through subtle cues in the environment rather than explicit stereotype activation [ 58 , 82 ]. It is therefore plausible that expectancies regarding performance may be further undermined when stigmatized in-group members are required to perform a stereotype-relevant task in front of out-group members. Advancing this suggestion, Sekaquaptewa and Thompson [ 82 ] examined the interactive effects of solo status and stereotype threat on women’s mathematical performance. Results revealed that women underperformed when they completed a quantitative examination in the presence of men (solo status) and under stereotype threat. However, whilst performance expectancies partially mediated the relationship between group composition and mathematical ability, they did not mediate the effects of stereotype threat on performance.

Explicit stereotype endorsement

Research has examined whether targeted individuals’ personal endorsement of negative stereotypes is associated with underperformance. For example, Leyens and colleagues [ 83 ] found that men underperformed on an affective task when they were told that they were not as apt as women in processing affective information. Against predictions, however, stereotype endorsement was not found to be a significant intermediary between stereotype threat and performance. Other studies also indicate that stereotype endorsement is not an underlying mechanism of the effects of self-as-target [ 3 ] and group-as-target stereotype threat on women’s mathematical aptitude [ 11 , 84 ].

Self-efficacy

Research suggests that self-efficacy can have a significant impact on an individual’s motivation and performance [ 85 , 86 , 87 ], and may be influenced by environmental cues [ 88 ]. Accordingly, it has been proposed that the situational salience of a negative stereotype may reduce an individual’s self-efficacy. As mentioned, Chung et al. [ 73 ] found that state anxiety and specific self-efficacy accounted for deficits in African American’s performance on a job promotion exam. However, additional studies indicate that self-efficacy does not mediate the effects of self-as-target threat on African American’s cognitive ability [ 72 ] and group-as-target threat on women’s mathematical performance [ 11 ].

Cognitive Mechanisms

Much research has proposed that affective and subjective variables underpin the harmful effects that stereotype threat exerts on performance [ 89 ]. However, other research posits that stereotype threat may influence performance detriments through its demands on cognitive processes [ 2 , 89 , 90 ]. Specifically, researchers have examined whether stereotype threat is mediated by; working memory, cognitive load, thought suppression, mind-wandering, negative thinking, cognitive appraisals and implicit stereotype endorsement.

Working memory

Schmader and Johns [ 89 ] proposed that performance-evaluative situations might reduce working memory capacity as stereotype-related thoughts consume cognitive resources. In three studies, they examined whether working memory accounted for the influence of a group-as-target threat on women’s and Latino American’s mathematical ability. Findings indicated that both female and Latino American participants solved fewer mathematical problems compared to participants in a non-threat control condition. Furthermore, reduced working memory capacity, measured via an operation span task [ 91 ], mediated the deleterious effects of stereotype threat on math performance. Supporting this, Rydell et al. (Experiment 3 [ 92 ]) found that working memory mediated the effects of a group-relevant stereotype on women’s mathematical performance when they perceived their performance to be evaluated in line with their gender identity. Here results also showed that these performance decrements were eliminated when women were concurrently primed with a positive and negative social identity (Experiment 2).

Further research has also examined how stereotype threat may simultaneously operate through cognitive and emotional processes. Across four experiments, Johns et al. [ 90 ] found that stereotype threat was accountable for deficits in women’s verbal, intellectual and mathematical ability. Moreover, emotion regulation − characterized as response-focused coping − mediated the effects of group-as-target stereotype threat on performance by depleting executive resources.

Nonetheless, executive functioning is made up of more cognitive processes than the construct of working memory [ 93 ]. Acknowledging this, Rydell et al. [ 93 ] predicted that updating (i.e., the ability to maintain and update information in the face of interference) would mediate stereotype threat effects. They further hypothesized that inhibition (i.e., the ability to inhibit a dominant response) and shifting (i.e., people’s ability to switch between tasks) should not underpin this effect. Results indicated that women who experienced an explicit group-as-target threat displayed reduced mathematical performance compared to a control condition. Consistent with predictions, only updating mediated the stereotype threat-performance relationship. These results suggest that the verbal ruminations associated with a negative stereotype may interfere with women’s ability to maintain and update the calculations needed to solve difficult math problems.

The extent to which updating accounts for stereotype threat effects in diverse populations, however, is less straightforward. For example, Hess et al. [ 32 ] found that working memory, measured by a computational span task, did not predict the relationship between group-based stereotype threat and older participants’ memory performance.

Cognitive load

There is ample evidence to suggest that stereotype threat depletes performance by placing higher demands on mental resources [ 89 , 93 ]. These demands may exert additional peripheral activity (i.e., emotional regulation) that can further interfere with task performance [ 90 ]. In order to provide additional support for this notion, Croizet et al. [ 94 ] examined whether increased mental load, measured by participants’ heart rate, mediated the effects of stereotype threat on Psychology majors’ cognitive ability. Here, Psychology majors were primed that they had lower intelligence compared to Science majors. Results indicated that this group-as-target stereotype threat undermined Psychology majors’ cognitive ability by triggering a psychophysiological mental load. Moreover, this increased mental load mediated the effects of stereotype threat on cognitive performance.

Thought suppression

Research suggests that individuals who experience stereotype threat may be aware that their performance will be evaluated in terms of a negative stereotype and, resultantly, engage in efforts to disprove it [ 3 , 94 , 95 ]. This combination of awareness and avoidance may lead to attempts to suppress negative thoughts that consequently tax the cognitive resources needed to perform effectively. In four experiments, Logel et al. (Experiment 2 [ 95 ]) examined whether stereotype threat influences stereotypical thought suppression by counterbalancing whether participants completed a stereotype-relevant lexical decision task before or after a mathematical test. Results indicated that women underperformed on the test in comparison to men. Interestingly, women tended to suppress stereotypical words when the lexical decision task was administered before the math test, but showed post-suppression rebound of stereotype-relevant words when this task was completed afterwards. Mediational analyses revealed that only pre-test thought suppression partially mediated the effects of stereotype threat on performance.

Mind-wandering

Previous research suggests that the anticipation of a stereotype-laden test may produce a greater proportion of task-related thoughts and worries [ 93 , 95 ]. Less research has examined the role of thoughts unrelated to the task in hand as a potential mediator of stereotype threat effects. Directly testing this notion, Mrazek et al. (Experiment 2 [ 74 ]) found that a group-as-target stereotype threat hampered women’s mathematical performance in comparison to a control condition. Furthermore, although self-report measures of mind-wandering resulted in null findings, indirect measures revealed that women under stereotype threat showed a marked decrease in attention. Mediation analyses indicated further that stereotype threat heightened anxiety which, in turn, increased mind-wandering and contributed to the observed impairments in math performance. Despite these findings, other studies have found no indication that task irrelevant thoughts mediate the effects of group-as-target stereotype threat on women’s mathematical performance [ 24 ] and African American participants’ cognitive ability [ 72 ].

Negative thinking

Schmader and Johns’ [ 89 ] research suggests that the performance deficits observed under stereotype threat may be influenced by intrusive thoughts. Further research [ 74 ] has included post-experimental measures of cognitive interference to assess the activation of distracting thoughts under stereotype threat. However, the content of these measures are predetermined by the experimenter and do not allow participants to report spontaneously on their experiences under stereotype threat. Overcoming these issues, Cadinu and colleagues [ 96 ] asked women to list their current thoughts whilst taking a difficult math test under conditions of stereotype threat. Results revealed that female participants underperformed when they perceived a mathematical test to be diagnostic of gender differences. Moreover, participants in the stereotype threat condition listed more negative thoughts relative to those in the control condition, with intrusive thoughts mediating the relationship between stereotype threat and poor math performance. It seems therefore that negative performance-related thoughts may consume working memory resources to impede performance.

Cognitive appraisal

Other research suggests that individuals may engage in coping strategies to offset the performance implications of a negative stereotype. One indicator of coping is cognitive appraisal, whereby individuals evaluate the significance of a situation as well as their ability to control it [ 97 ]. Here, individuals may exert more effort on a task when the situational presents as a challenge, but may disengage from the task if they evaluate the situation as a threat [ 98 , 99 ]. Taking this into consideration, Berjot, Roland-Levy and Girault-Lidvan [ 100 ] proposed that targeted members might be more likely to perceive a negative stereotype as a threat to their group identity rather than as a challenge to disprove it. They found that North African secondary school students underperformed on a visuospatial task when they perceived French students to possess superior perceptual-motor skills. Contrary to predictions, threat appraisal did not mediate the relation between stereotype threat and performance. Rather, perceiving the situation as a challenge significantly mediated the stereotype threat-performance relationship. Specifically, participants who appraised stereotype threat as a challenge performed better than those who did not. These results therefore suggest that individuals may strive to confront, rather than avoid, intellectual challenges and modify the stereotype held by members of a relevant out-group in a favorable direction [ 101 ].

Implicit stereotype endorsement

Situational cues that present as a threat may increase the activation of automatic associations between a stereotyped concept (i.e., female), negative attributes (i.e., bad), and the performance domain (i.e., math; [ 102 ]). Implicit measures may be able to detect recently formed automatic associations between concepts and stereotypical attributes that are not yet available to explicitly self-report [ 103 ]. In a study of 240 six-year old children, Galdi et al. [ 103 ] examined whether implicit stereotype threat endorsement accounted for the effects of stereotype threat on girls’ mathematical performance. Consistent with the notion that automatic associations can precede conscious beliefs, results indicated that girls acquire implicit math-gender stereotypes before they emerge at an explicit level. Specifically, girls showed stereotype-consistent automatic associations between the terms ‘boy-mathematics’ and ‘girl-language’, which mediated stereotype threat effects.

Motivational Mechanisms

Most of the initial work on the underlying mechanisms of stereotype threat has focused on affective and cognitive processes. More recently, research has begun to examine whether individuals may be motivated to disconfirm a negative stereotype, with this having a paradoxical effect of harming performance [ 104 , 105 , 106 ]. To this end, research has elucidated the potential role of effort, self-handicapping, dejection, vigilance, and achievement goals.

Effort/motivation

Underpinned by the “mere effort model” [ 104 ], Jamieson and Harkins [ 105 ] examined whether motivation plays a proximal role in the effect of stereotype threat on women’s math performance. Here they predicted that stereotype threat would lead participants to use a conventional problem solving approach (i.e., use known equations to compute an answer), which would facilitate performance on ‘solve’ problems, but hamper performance on ‘comparison’ problems. Results supported this hypothesis, indicating that stereotype threat debilitated performance on comparison problems as participants employed the dominant, but incorrect, solution approach. Furthermore, this incorrect solving approach mediated the effect of stereotype threat on comparison problem performance. This suggests that stereotype threat motivates participants to perform well, which increases activation of a dominant response to the task. However, as this dominant approach does not always guarantee success, the work indicates that different problem solving strategies may determine whether a person underperforms on a given task [ 105 , 107 ].

Stereotype threat may have differential effects on effort dependent on the prime utilized [ 27 ]. For example, Skorich et al. [ 27 ] examined whether effort mediated the effects of implicit and explicit stereotypes on provisional drivers’ performance on a hazard perception test. Participants in the implicit prime condition ticked their driving status (provisional, licensed) on a questionnaire, whereas participants in the explicit prime condition were provided with stereotypes relating to the driving ability of provisional licensees. Results revealed that participants detected more hazards when they were primed with an explicit stereotype relative to an implicit stereotype. Mediational analyses showed that whilst increased effort mediated the effects of an implicit stereotype on performance, decreased effort mediated the effects of an explicit stereotype prime. Research also indicates that reduced effort mediates the effects of an explicit stereotype on older adults’ memory recall [ 18 ]. Taken together, these results suggest that implicit stereotype primes may lead to increased effort as participants aim to disprove the stereotype, whereas explicit stereotype threat primes may lead to decreased effort as participants self-handicap [ 27 ]. Nevertheless, other studies utilizing self-reported measures of effort have resulted in non-significant findings (Experiment 1 & 2 [ 14 ]; Experiment 4 [ 44 ]; Experiment 2 [ 77 ]; Experiment 2, 4 & 5, [ 108 ]).

Self-handicapping

Individuals may engage in self-handicapping strategies to proactively reduce the applicability of a negative stereotype to their performance. Here, people attempt to influence attributions for performance by erecting barriers to their success. Investigating this notion, Stone [ 15 ] examined whether self-handicapping mediated the effects of stereotype threat on white athletes’ sporting performance. Self-handicapping was measured by the total amount of stereotype-relevant words completed on a word-fragment task. Results indicated that white athletes practiced less when they perceived their ability on a golf-putting task to be diagnostic of personal ability, thereby confirming a negative stereotype relating to ‘poor white athleticism’. Moreover, these athletes were more likely to complete the term ‘awkward’ on a word fragment completion test compared to the control condition. Mediation analyses revealed that the greater accessibility of the term ‘awkward’ partially mediated the effects of stereotype threat on psychological disengagement and performance. The authors suggest that stereotype threat increased the accessibility of thoughts related to poor athleticism to inhibit athletes' practice efforts. However, a limitation of this research is that analyses were based on single-item measures (i.e., the completion of the word ‘awkward’) rather than total of completed words on the word-fragment test.

Keller [ 109 ] also tested the hypothesis that the salience of a negative stereotype is related to self-handicapping tendencies. Results showed that women who were primed with a group-as-target stereotype underperformed on a mathematical test relative to their control group counterparts. Furthermore, they expressed stronger tendencies to search for external explanations for their weak performance with this mediating the effects of stereotype threat on performance. Despite these preliminary findings, Keller and Dauenheimer [ 44 ] were unable to provide support for the notion that self-reported self-handicapping is a significant intermediary between stereotype threat and women’s mathematical underperformance.

Research on performance expectations suggests that stereotype threat effects may be mediated by goals set by the participants. Extending this work, Keller and Dauenheimer [ 44 ] hypothesized that female participants may make more errors on a mathematical test due to an overly motivated approach strategy. Results indicated that women underperformed when a math test was framed as diagnostic of gender differences (a group-as-target threat). Furthermore, their experiences of dejection were found to mediate the relation between stereotype threat and performance. The authors suggest that individuals may be motivated to disconfirm the negative stereotype and thus engage in a promotion focus of self-regulation. However, feelings of failure may elicit an emotional response that resultantly determines underperformance.

In contrast to Keller and Dauenheimer [ 44 ], Seibt and Förster (Experiment 5; [ 108 ]) proposed that under stereotype threat, targeted individuals engage in avoidance and vigilance strategies. They predicted that positive stereotypes should induce a promotion focus, leading to explorative and creative processing, whereas negative stereotypes should induce a prevention focus state of vigilance, with participants avoiding errors. Across five experiments, male and female participants were primed with a group-as-target stereotype suggesting that women have better verbal abilities than men. However, rather than showing a stereotype threat effect, results indicated a speed-accuracy trade off with male participants completing an analytical task slower but more accurately than their counterparts in a non-threat control condition. Furthermore, this prevention focus of vigilance was found to partially mediate the effects of stereotype threat on men’s analytical abilities (Experiment 5). The authors conclude that the salience of a negative group stereotype elicits a vigilant, risk-averse processing style that diminishes creativity and speed while bolstering analytic thinking and accuracy.

Achievement goals

Achievement goals theory [ 110 ] posits that participants will evaluate their role in a particular achievement context and endorse either performance-focused or performance-avoidance goals. In situations where the chances of success are low, individuals engage in performance-avoidance goals, corresponding to a desire to avoid confirming a negative stereotype. Accordingly, Chalabaev et al. [ 111 ] examined whether performance avoidance goals mediated the effects of stereotype threat on women’s sporting performance. Here, the impact of two self-as-target stereotypes (i.e., poor athletic and soccer ability) on performance were assessed relative to a control condition. Results indicated that women in the athletic ability condition performed more poorly on a dribbling task, but not in the soccer ability condition. Furthermore, although these participants endorsed a performance-avoidance goal, this did not mediate the relationship between stereotype threat and soccer performance.

Highlighting the possible interplay between affective, cognitive and motivation mechanisms, Brodish and Devine [ 112 ] proffered a multi-mediator model, proposing that anxiety and performance-avoidance goals may mediate the effects of group-as-target stereotype threat on women’s mathematical performance. Achievement goals were measured by whether participants endorsed performance-avoidant (the desire to avoid performing poorly) or approach goals (trying to outperform others). Results indicated that women under stereotype threat solved fewer mathematical problems relative to those in a control condition. Mediation analyses revealed that performance avoidance goals and anxiety sequentially mediated women’s mathematical performance. That is, stereotype threatened women were motivated to avoid failure, which in turn heightened anxiety and influenced underperformance. Table 2 summarizes the articles reviewed and details their key findings and respective methodologies. See S2 Table for overview of significant mediational findings.

Table 2. Summary of stereotype threat literature examining mediational variables with key methodologies and findings.

The current review evaluated empirical support for the mediators of stereotype threat. Capitalizing on the multi-threat framework [ 31 ], we distinguished between self-relevant and group-relevant stereotype threats to examine the extent to which these are mediated by qualitatively distinct mechanisms and imperil diverse stigmatized populations. On the whole, the results of the current review indicate that experiences of stereotype threat may increase individuals’ feelings of anxiety, negative thinking and mind-wandering which deplete the working memory resources required for successful task execution. Research documents further that individuals may be motivated to disconfirm the negative stereotype and engage in efforts to suppress stereotypical thoughts that are inconsistent with task goals. However, many of the mediators tested have resulted in varying degrees of empirical support. Below we suggest that stereotype threat may operate in distinct ways dependent on the population under study, the primes utilized, and the instruments used to measure mediation and performance.

Previous research has largely conceptualized stereotype threat as a singular construct, experienced similarly by individuals and groups across situations [ 31 , 55 ]. Consequently, research has overlooked the possibility of multiple forms of stereotype threats that may be implicated through concerns to an individual’s personal or social identity [ 31 ]. This is highlighted in the present review, as the majority of stereotype threat studies employed a group-as-target prime. Here stereotype threat is typically instantiated to highlight that stereotype-consistent performance may confirm, or reinforce, a negative societal stereotype as being a true representation of one’s social group [ 48 ]. This has led to a relative neglect of situations in which individuals may anticipate that their performance may be indicative of personal ability [ 31 , 55 ].

Similar processes such as arousal, deficits in working memory, and motivation may be triggered by self-as-target and group-as-target stereotype threats. However, it is important to note that the experiences of these stereotype threats may be fundamentally distinct [ 31 ]. That is, deficits in working memory under self-as-target stereotype threat may be evoked by negative thoughts relating to the self (i.e., ruining one’s opportunities, letting oneself down). Conversely, group-based intrusive thoughts may mediate the effects of group-as-target threat on performance as individuals view their performance in line with their social group (i.e., confirming a societal stereotype, letting the group down) [ 31 ]. Moreover, research suggests that when a group-based stereotype threat is primed, individuals dissociate their sense of self from the negatively stereotyped domain [ 78 ]. Yet, this may be more unlikely when an individual experiences self-as-target stereotype threat as their personal ability is explicitly tied to a negative stereotype that governs their ingroup. As such, the activation of a group-based stereotype may set in motion mechanisms that reflect a protective orientation of self-regulation, whereas self-relevant knowledge may heighten self-consciousness. To date, however, research has not explicitly distinguished between self-as-target and group-as-target stereotype threat in the elucidation of mediating variables. Future research would therefore benefit from a systematic investigation of how different stereotype threats may hamper performance in qualitatively distinguishable ways. One way to investigate the hypotheses set out here would be to allow participants to spontaneously report their experiences under self-as-target and group-as-target stereotype threat, and to examine differences in the content of participants’ thoughts as a function of these different primes.

In a similar vein, different mechanisms may mediate the effects of blatant and subtle stereotype threat effects on performance [ 27 , 58 , 111 ]. Blatant threat manipulations explicitly inform participants of a negative stereotype related to performance (e.g., [ 3 , 11 ]), whereas placing stigmatized group members in a situation in which they have minority status may evoke more subtle stereotype threat [ 78 , 82 ]. Providing evidence consistent with this notion, Sekaquaptewa and Thompson [ 82 ] found that performance expectancies partially mediated the effects of solo status, but not stereotype threat on performance. These results suggest that women may make comparative judgments about their expected performance when they are required to undertake an exam in the presence of out-group members, yet may not consciously recognize how a negative stereotype can directly impair performance. Further research suggests that working memory may mediate the effects of subtle stereotype threat cues on performance as individuals attend to situational cues that heighten the salience of a discredited identity [ 88 , 94 ]. Alternatively, motivation may mediate the effects of blatant stereotype threat as individuals strive to disprove the negative stereotype [ 27 , 44 , 58 , 108 ]. Although stereotype threat effects appear to be robust [ 30 ], it is plausible that these distinct manipulations diverge in the nature, the focus, and the intensity of threat they produce and may therefore be mediated by different mechanisms [ 31 ].

It is also conceivable that different groups are more susceptible to certain types of stereotype threat [ 13 , 31 , 56 ]. For example, research indicates that women’s performance on a social cognition task was influenced to a greater extent by implicit gender-related stereotypes, whereas men were more vulnerable to explicit stereotype threat [ 13 ]. Further research suggests that populations who tend to have low group identification (e.g., those with a mental illness or obesity) are more susceptible to self-as-target threats. Conversely, populations with high group identification, such as individuals of a certain ethnicity, gender or religion are more likely to experience group-as-target threats [ 56 ]. Whilst this highlights the role of moderating variables that heighten individuals’ susceptibility to stereotype threat, it also suggests that individuals may experience stereotype threat in different ways, dependent on their stigmatized identity. This may explain why some variables (e.g., anxiety, self-handicapping) that have been found to mediate the effects of stereotype threat on some groups have not emerged in other populations.

Finally, it is conceivable that diverse mediators account for the effects of stereotype threat on different performance outcomes. For example, although working memory is implicated in tasks that typically require controlled processing, it is not required for tasks that rely more on automatic processes [ 24 , 58 , 93 ]. In line with this notion, Beilock et al. [ 24 ] found that experts’ golf putting skills were harmed under stereotype threat when attention was allocated to automatic processes that operate usually outside of working memory. This suggests that well-learned skills may be hampered by attempts to bring performance back under step-by-step control. Conversely, skills such as difficult math problem solving appear to involve heavy processing demands and may be harmed when working memory is consumed by a negative stereotype. As such, distinct mechanisms may underpin different threat-related performance outcomes.

Limitations of Stereotype Threat Research

We now outline methodological issues in current stereotype threat literature with a view to inform the design of future research. First, researchers have predominantly utilized self-report measures in their efforts to uncover the mediating variables of stereotype threat. However, it has long been argued that individuals have limited access to higher order mental processes [ 113 , 114 ], such as those involved in the evaluation and initiation of behavior [ 115 , 116 ]. Resultantly, participants under stereotype threat may be unable to observe and explicitly report the operations of their own mind [ 29 , 114 , 117 , 118 , 119 ]. Consistent with this assertion, Bosson et al. [ 29 ] found that although stereotype threat heightened individuals’ physiological anxiety, the same individuals did not report an awareness of increased anxiety on self-report measures. Participants may thus be mindful of the impression they make on others and engage in self-presentational behaviors in an effort to appear invulnerable to negative stereotypes [ 29 ]. This is supported by research suggesting that stereotype threatened participants tend not to explicitly endorse stereotypes [ 29 , 37 , 83 , 84 ] and are more likely to claim impediments to justify poor performance [ 3 , 14 , 109 ]. Moreover, it is possible that stereotype threat processes are non-conscious [ 119 ] with research indicating that implicit–but not explicit–stereotype endorsement mediates stereotype threat effects [ 103 ]. This suggests that non-conscious processing of stereotype-relevant information may influence the decrements observed in individuals’ performance under stereotype threat. Furthermore, this research underscores the greater sensitivity of indirect measures for examining the mediators of stereotype threat. From this perspective, future research may benefit from the use of physiological measures, such as heart rate, cortisol and skin conductance to examine anxiety (c.f., [ 94 , 120 , 121 ]), the IAT to measure implicit stereotype endorsement [ 103 ] and the sustained response to attention task to measure mind-wandering [ 74 ].

In the investigation of stereotype threat, self-report measures may be particularly susceptible to order effects. For example, Brodish and Devine [ 112 ] found that women reported higher levels of anxiety when they completed a questionnaire before a mathematical test compared to afterwards. This suggests that pre-test anxiety ratings may have reflected participants’ uneasiness towards the upcoming evaluative test, with this apprehension diminishing once the test was completed. Research by Logel and colleagues [ 95 ] provides support for this notion, indicating that women who completed a lexical decision task after a math test were quicker to respond to stereotype-relevant words compared to women who subsequently completed the task. These results exhibit the variability in individuals’ emotions under stereotype threat and suggest that they may be unable to retrospectively report on their feelings once the threat has passed. This emphasizes the importance of counterbalancing test instruments in the investigation of stereotype threat, purporting that the order in which test materials are administered may influence mediational findings.

This review highlights that, in some studies, individuals assigned to a control condition may have also experienced stereotype threat, thus potentially preventing reliable evidence of mediation. For instance, Chalabaev et al. [ 111 ] primed stereotype threat by presenting a soccer ability test as a diagnostic indicator of personal factors related to athletic ability. Nevertheless, participants in the control condition received information that the aim of the test was to examine psychological factors in athletic ability. Consequently, these participants may have also been apprehensive about their performance being evaluated, and this may have precluded evidence that achievement goals mediate the stereotype threat-performance relationship. Furthermore, research has manipulated the salience of stereotype threat by stating that gender differences in math performance are equal [ 82 ]. However, other research has utilized this prime within control conditions (e.g., [ 94 , 105 , 119 ]), underpinned by the rationale that describing a test as ‘fair’ or non-diagnostic of ability eliminates stereotype threat [ 122 ]. It is therefore possible that, in some instances, researchers have inadvertently induced stereotype threat. This outlines the importance of employing a control condition in which individuals are not made aware of any negative stereotypes, and are told that the test is non-diagnostic of ability, in order to detect possible mediators.

Two decades of research have demonstrated the harmful effects that stereotype threat can exert on a wide range of populations in a broad array of performance domains. However, findings with regards to the mediators that underpin these effects are equivocal. This may be a consequence of the heterogeneity of primes used to instantiate stereotype threat and the methods used to measure mediation and performance. To this end, future work is likely to benefit from the following directions: First, account for the existence of multiple stereotype threats; Second, recognize that the experiences of stereotype threat may differ between stigmatized groups, and that no one mediator may provide generalized empirical support across diverse populations; Third, utilize indirect measures, in addition to self-report measures, to examine reliably mediating variables and to examine further the convergence of these two methods; Fourth, counterbalance test instruments to control for order effects; and finally, ensure that participants in a control condition do not inadvertently encounter stereotype threat by stating explicitly that the task is non-diagnostic of ability.

Supporting Information

Data availability.

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The authors acknowledge support toward open access publishing by the Graduate School and the Department of Psychology at Edge Hill University. The funders had no role in the systematic review, data collection or analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

  • 1. Derks B, Inzlicht M, Kang S. The neuroscience of stigma and stereotype threat. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2008;11: 163–181. 10.1177/136843020708803 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 2. Schmader T, Johns M, Forbes C. An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychol Rev. 2008;115: 336–356. 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 3. Steele CM, Aronson J. Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69: 797–811. 10.1037/0022-3514/0022-3514.69.5.797 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 4. Devine PG, Brodish AB. Modern classics in social psychology. Psychol Inq. 2003;14: 196–202. 10.1080/1047840X.2003.9682879 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 5. Fiske ST. The discomfort index: How to spot a really good idea whose time has come. Psychological Inquiry. 2003;14: 203–208. 10.1080/1047840X.2003.9682880 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 6. Google Scholar. Citation Reports. Available: https://scholar.google.co.uk/scholar?cites=15396601321629817023&as_sdt=2005&sciodt=0,5&hl=en
  • 7. Benbow CP, Stanley JC. Sex differences in mathematical ability: Fact or artifact? Science. 1980;210: 1262–1264. 10.1126/science.7434028 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 8. Spelke ES. Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science? A critical review. Am Psychol. 2005;60: 950–958. 10.1037/0003-066X.60.9.950 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 9. Sternberg RJ, Grigorenko EL, Kidd KK. Intelligence, race, and genetics. Am Psychol. 2006;60: 46–59. 10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 10. Steele CM. A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. Am Psychol. 1997;52: 613–629. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 11. Spencer SJ, Steele CM, Quinn DM. Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1999;35: 4–28. 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 12. Stoet G, Geary DC. Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematical performance and achievement? Rev Gen Psychol. 2012;16: 93–102. 10.1037/a0026617 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 13. Pavlova MA, Weber S, Simoes E, Sokolov AN. Gender Stereotype Susceptibility. PLOS ONE; 2014;9(12): e114802 10.1371/journal.pone.0114802 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 14. Aronson J, Lustina MJ, Good C, Keough K, Steele CM, Brown J. When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1999;35: 29–46. 10.1006/jesp.1998.1371 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 15. Stone J. Battling doubt by avoiding practice: The effects of stereotype threat on self-handicapping in White athletes. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2002;28: 1667–1678. 10.1177/014616702237648 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 16. Stone J, Lynch CI, Sjomeling M, Darley JM. Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;77: 1213–1227. 10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 17. Walton GM, Spencer SJ. Latent ability: Grades and test scores systematically underestimate intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students. Psychol Sci. 2009;20: 1132–1139. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 18. Hess TM, Auman C, Colcombe SJ, Rahhal TA. The impact of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2003;58: 3–11. 10.1093/geronb/58.1.P3 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 19. Levy BR. Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71: 1092–1107. 10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1092 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 20. Moè A. Gender difference does not mean genetic difference: Externalizing improves performance in mental rotation. Learn Individ Differ. 2012;22: 20–24. 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.001 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 21. Moè A, Pazagglia F. Following the instructions! Effects of gender beliefs in mental rotation. Learn Individ Differ. 2006:16; 369–377. 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.01.002 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 22. Moè A, Pazagglia F. Beyond genetics in mental rotation test performance: The power of effort attribution. Learn Individ Differ. 2010:5; 464–468. 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.03.004 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 23. Wraga M, Duncan L, Jacobs EC, Helt M, Church J. Stereotype susceptibility narrows the gender gap in imagined self-rotation performance. Psychol Bull Rev. 2006;13: 813–819. 10.3758/BF03194002 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 24. Beilock SL, Rydell RJ, McConnell AR. Stereotype threat and working memory: Mechanisms, alleviation, and spill-over. J Exp Psychol: Gen. 2007;136: 256–276. 10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.256 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 25. Schuster C, Martiny SE, Schmader T. Distracted by the unthought–Suppression and reappraisal of mind wandering under stereotype threat. PLOS ONE. 2015;10(3): e0122207 10.1371/journal.pone.0122207 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 26. Beilock SL, Jellison WA, Rydell RJ, McConnell AR, Carr TH. On the causal mechanisms of stereotype threat: Can skills that don’t rely heavily on working memory still be threatened? Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2006;32: 1059–1071. 10.1177/0146167206288489 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 27. Skorich DP, Webb H, Stewart L, Kostyanaya M, Cruwyz T, McNeill K, et al. Stereotype threat and hazard perception among provisional license drivers. Accid Anal Prev. 2013;54: 39–45. 10.1016/j.aap.2013.02.002 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 28. Yeung NC, von Hippel C. Stereotype threat increases the likelihood that female drivers in a simulator run over jaywalkers. Accid Anal Prev. 2008;40: 667–674. 10.1016/j.aap.2007.09.003 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 29. Bosson JK, Haymovitz EL, Pinel EC. When saying and doing diverge: The effect of stereotype threat on self-reported versus non-verbal anxiety. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2004;40: 247–255. 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00099-4 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 30. Nguyen H-HD, Ryan AM. Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Appl Psychol. 2008;93: 1314–1334. 10.1037/a0012702 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 31. Shapiro JR, Neuberg SL. From stereotype threat to stereotype threats: Implications of a multi-threat framework for causes, moderators, mediators, consequences, and interventions. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2007;11: 107–130. 10.1177/1088868306294790 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 32. Hess TM, Hinson JT, Hodges EA. Moderators of and mechanisms underlying stereotype threat effects on older adults’ memory performance. Exp Aging Res. 2009;35: 153–177. 10.1080/03610730802716413 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 33. Keller J. Stereotype threat in classroom settings: The interactive effect of domain identification, task difficulty and stereotype threat on female students’ maths performance. Br J Educ Psychol. 2007;77: 323–328. 10.1348/000709906X113662 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 34. Régner I, Smeding A, Gimmig D, Thinus-Blanc C, Monteil J-M, Huguet P. Individual differences in working memory moderate stereotype-threat effects. Psychol Sci. 2010;21: 1646–1648. 10.1177/0956797610386619 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 35. Brown RP, Pinel EC. Stigma on my mind: Individual differences in the experience of stereotype threat. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2003;39: 626–633. 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00039-8 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 36. Elizaga DA, Markman KD. Peers and performance: How in-group and out-group comparisons moderate stereotype threat effects. Curr Psychol. 2008;27: 290–300. 10.1007/s12144-008-9041-y [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 37. Schmader T, Johns M, Barquissau M. The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles. 2004;50: 835–850. 10.1023/B:SERS.0000029101.74557.a0 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 38. Rydell RJ, Boucher KL. Capitalizing on multiple social identities to prevent stereotype threat: The moderating role of self-esteem. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2010:36; 239–250. 10.1177/0146167209355062 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 39. Cadinu M, Maass A, Lombardo M, Frigerio S. Stereotype threat: The moderating role of locus of control beliefs. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2006;36: 183–197. 10.1002/ejsp.303 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 40. Davies C, Aronson J, Salinas M. Shades of threat: Racial identity as a moderator of stereotype threat. J Black Psychol. 2006;32: 399–417. 10.1177/0095798406292464 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 41. Marx DM, Stapel DA, Muller D. We can do it: The interplay of construal orientation and social comparison under threat. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88: 432–446. 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.432 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 42. Schmader J. Gender identification moderates stereotype threat on women’s math performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2002;38: 194–201. 10.1006/jesp.2001.1500 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 43. Appel M, Kronberger N, Aronson J. Stereotype threat impairs ability building: Effects on test preparation among women in science and technology. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2011;41: 904–913. 10.1002/ejsp.835 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 44. Keller J, Dauenheimer D. Stereotype threat in the classroom: Dejection mediates the disrupting effect of women’s math performance. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29: 371–381. 10.1177/0146167202250218 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 45. Oyserman D, Harrison K, Bybee D. Can racial identity be promotive of academic efficacy? Int J Behav Dev. 2001;25: 379–385. 10.1080/01650250042000401 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 46. Oyserman D, Kemmelmeier M, Fryberg S, Brosh H, Hart-Johnson T. Racial-ethnic self-schemas. Soc Psychol Quart. 2003;66: 333–347. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 47. Smith JL. Understanding the process of stereotype threat: A review of mediational variables and new performance goal directions. Educ Psychol Rev. 2004;16: 177–206. 10.1023/B:EDPR.0000034020.20317.89 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 48. Steele CM, Spencer SJ, Aronson J. Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat In: Zanna M, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34). New York, NY: Academic Press; 2002. pp. 379–440. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 49. Crocker J, Major B. Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychol Rev. 1989;96: 608–630. 10.1037/0033-295x.96.4.608 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 50. Tajfel H, Turner JC. The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour In: Worchel S, Austin WG, editors. Psychology of intergroup relations. Chicago: Nelson-Hall; 1986. pp. 7–24. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 51. Frey FE, Tropp LR. Being seen as individuals versus as group members: Extending research on metaperception to intergroup contexts. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2006;10: 265–280. 10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_5 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 52. Leary MR, Terry ML, Allen A, Tate EB. The concept of ego threat in social and personality psychology: Is ego threat a viable scientific construct? Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2009;13: 151–164. 10.1177/1088868309342595 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 53. Wheeler SC, Demarree KG, Petty RE. Understanding the role of the self in prime-to-behavior effects: The active-self account. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2007;11: 234–261. 10.1177/1088868307302223 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 54. Hirsh JB, Kang SK. Mechanisms of identity conflict: Uncertainty, anxiety and the behavioral inhibition system. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2015; Advanced online article. 10.1177/1088868315589475 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 55. Shapiro JR, Williams AM, Hambarchyan M. Are all interventions created equal? A multi-threat approach to tailoring stereotype threat interventions. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2013;104: 277–288. 10.1037/a0030461 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 56. Shapiro JR. Different groups, different threats: A multi-threat approach to the experience of stereotype threats. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2011;37: 464–480. 10.1177/0146167211398140 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 57. Shapiro JR. Types of threats: From stereotype threat to stereotype threats In: Schmader T, Inzlicht M, editors. Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 71–88. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 58. Stone J, McWhinnie C. Evidence that blatant versus subtle stereotype threat cues impact performance through dual processes. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2008;44: 445–452. 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.02.006 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 59. Dickersin K. Publication bias: Recognizing the problem, understanding its origins and scope, and preventing harm In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons; 2005. pp. 11–33. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 60. Dickersin K, Min Y-I, Meinert CL. Factors influencing publication of research results. Follow-up of applications submitted to two institutional review boards. JAMA. 1992;267: 374–378. 10.1001/jama.1992.03480030052036 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 61. Rosenthal R. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol Bull. 1979;86: 638–641. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 62. Moher D, Liberato A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLOS Med. 2009;6: e1000097 10.1371/journal.pmed1000097 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 63. Cronin P, Ryan F, Coughlan M. Undertaking a literature review: A step-by-step approach. Brit J Nurs. 2008;17: 38–43. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 64. Khan KS, Riet GT, Popay J, Nixon J, Kleijnen J. (2009). Study quality assessment (Stage 2 Conducting the review, Phase 5). In: Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (Ed.), Systematic Reviews. pp. 1–20. Available: http://www.medicine.mcgill.ca/rtamblyn/Readings%5CUK%20Guide%20-%20Study%20Quality%20Assessment.pdf
  • 65. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach New York: Guildford Press; 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 66. Preacher KJ, Kelley K. Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychol Methods. 2011;16: 93–115. 10.1037/a0022658 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 67. Rucker DD, Preacher KJ, Tormala ZL, Petty RE. Mediation analysis in social psychology: Current practices and new recommendations. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2011;5/6: 359–371. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00355.x [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 68. Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, Hayes AF. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivar Behav Res. 2007;42: 185–227. 10.1080/00273170701341316 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 69. Aronson J, Inzlicht M. The ups and downs of attributional ambiguity: Stereotype vulnerability and the academic self-knowledge of African-American students. Psychol Sci. 2004;15: 829–836. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00763.x [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 70. Kray LJ, Thompson L, Galinsky A. Battle of the sexes: Gender stereotype confirmation and reactance in negotiations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80: 942–958. 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.942 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 71. Tempel T, Neumann R. Stereotype threat, test anxiety, and mathematics performance. Soc Psychol Educ. 2014;17: 491–501. 10.1007/s11218-014-9263-9 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 72. Mayer DM, Hanges PJ. Understanding the stereotype threat effect with “culture free” tests: An examination of its mediators and measurement. Hum Perf. 2003;16: 207–230. 10.1207/S15327043HUP1603_3 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 73. Chung BG, Ehrhart MG, Holcombe-Ehrhart K, Hattrup K, Solamon J. Stereotype threat, state anxiety, and specific self-efficacy as predictors of promotion exam performance. Group Organ Manage. 2010;35: 77–107. 10.1177/1059601109354839 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 74. Mrazek MD, Chin JM, Schmader T, Hartson KA, Smallwood J, Schooler JW. Threatened to distraction: Mind-wandering as a consequence of stereotype threat. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2011;47: 1243–1248. 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.011 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 75. Laurin R. Stereotype threat and lift effects in motor task performance: The mediating role of somatic and cognitive anxiety. J Soc Psychol. 2013;153: 687–699. 10.1080/00224545.2013.821098 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 76. Gerstenberg FXR, Imhoff R, Schmitt M. ‘Women are bad at math, but I’m not, am I?’ Fragile mathematical self-concept predicts vulnerability to a stereotype threat effect on mathematical performance. Eur J Pers. 2012;26: 588–599. 10.1002/per.1836 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 77. McKown C, Weinstein RS. The development and consequences of stereotype consciousness in middle childhood. Child Dev. 2003;74: 498–515. 10.1111/1467-8624.7402012 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 78. Keller J, Sekaquaptewa D. Solo status and women’s spatial test performance: The role of individuation tendencies. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2008;38: 1044–1053. 10.1002/ejsp.490 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 79. O’Brien LT, Crandall CS. Stereotype threat and arousal: Effects on women’s maths performance. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29: 782–789. 10.1177/0146167203029006010 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 80. Cadinu M, Maass A, Frigerio S, Impagliazzo L, Latinotti S. Stereotype threat: The effect of expectancy on performance. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2003;33: 267–285. 10.1002/ejsp.145 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 81. Rosenthal HES, Crisp RJ, Suen M-W. Improving performance expectancies in stereotypic domains: Task relevance and the reduction of stereotype threat. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2007:37; 586–597. 10.1002/ejsp.379 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 82. Sekaquaptewa D, Thompson M. Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2003;39: 68–74. 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00508-5 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 83. Leyens J-P, Désert M, Croizet J-C, Darcis C. Stereotype threat: Are lower status and history of stigmatization preconditions of stereotype threat? Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2000;26: 1189–1199. 10.1177/0146167200262002 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 84. Beaton A, Tougas F, Rinfret N, Huard N, Delisle M- N. Strength in numbers? Women and mathematics. Eur J of Psychol Educ. 2009;22: 291–306. 10.1007/BF03173427 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 85. Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1986. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 86. Maddux JE. Social cognitive models of health and exercise behavior: An introduction and review of conceptual issues. J Appl Sport Psychol. 1993;5: 116–140. 10.1080/10413209308411310 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 87. Schunk DH. Self-efficacy and achievement behaviors. Educ Psychol Rev. 1989;1: 173–208. 10.1007/BF01320134 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 88. Bandura A. Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2006;1: 164–180. 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00011.x [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 89. Schmader T, Johns M. Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85: 440–452. 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 90. Johns M, Inzlicht M, Schmader T. Stereotype threat and executive resource depletion: Examining the influence of emotion regulation. J Exp Psychol: Gen. 2008;137: 691–705. 10.1037/a0013834 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 91. Turner ML, Engle RW. Is working memory capacity task dependent? J Mem Lang. 1989;28: 127–154. 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90040-5 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 92. Rydell RJ, McConnelL AR, Beilock SL. Multiple social identities and stereotype threat: Imbalance, accessibility, and working memory. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;96: 949–966. 10.1037/a0014846 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 93. Rydell RJ, Van Loo KJ, Boucher KL. Stereotype threat and executive functions: Which functions mediate different threat-related outcomes? Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2014;40: 377–390. 10.1177/0146167213513475 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 94. Croizet J-C, Després G, Gauzins M-E, Huguet P, Leyens J-P, Méot A. Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004;30: 721–731. 10.1177/0146167204263961 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 95. Logel C, Iserman EC, Davies PG, Quinn DM, Spencer SJ. The perils of double consciousness: The role of thought suppression in stereotype threat. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;45: 299–312. 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.016 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 96. Cadinu M, Maass A, Rosabianca A, Kiesner J. Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of negative thinking. Psychol Sci. 2005;16: 572, 578. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01577.x [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 97. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping New York: Springer; 1984. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 98. Drach-Zahavy A, Erez M. Challenge versus threat effects on the goal-performance relationship. Organ Behav Human December 2002;88: 667–682. 10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00004-3 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 99. White JB. Fail or flourish? Cognitive appraisal moderates the effect of solo status on performance. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2008;34: 1171–1184. 10.1177/0146167208318404 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 100. Berjot S, Roland-Levy C, Girault-Lidvan N. Cognitive appraisals of stereotype threat. Psychol Rep. 2011;108: 585–598. 10.2466/04.07.21.PR0.108.2.585-598 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 101. Cohen GL, Garcia J. “I am us”: Negative stereotypes as collective threats. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;89: 566–582. 10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.566 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 102. Nosek BA, Banaji MR, Greenwald AG. Math = male, me = female, therefore math ≠ me. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83: 44–59. 10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.44 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 103. Galdi S, Cadinu M, Tomasetto C. The roots of stereotype threat: When automatic associations disrupt girls’ math performance. Child Dev. 2014;85: 250–263. 10.1111/cdev.12128 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 104. Jamieson JP, Harkins SD. Mere effort and stereotype threat performance effects. J Pers Soc Psychology. 2007;93: 544–564. 10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.544 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 105. Jamieson JP, Harkins SG. Distinguishing between the effects of stereotype priming and stereotype threat on math performance. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2011;15: 291–304. 10.1177/1368430211417833 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 106. Jamieson JP, Harkins SG. Mere effort and stereotype threat performance effects. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;93: 544–564. 10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.544 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 107. Moè A, Meneghetti C, Cadinu M. Women and mental rotation: Incremental theory and spatial strategy use to enhance performance. Pers Indiv Differ. 2009;46: 187–191. 10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.030 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 108. Seibt B, Förster J. Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;87: 38–56. 10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 109. Keller J. Blatant stereotype threat and women’s math performance: Self-handicapping as a strategic means to cope with obtrusive negative performance expectations. Sex Roles. 2002;47: 193–198. 10.1023/A:1021003307511 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 110. Elliot AJ, Church MA. A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72: 218–232. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 111. Chalabaev A, Sarrazin P, Stone J, Cury F. Do achievement goals mediate stereotype threat? An investigation on females’ soccer performance. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2008;20: 143–158. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 112. Brodish AB, Devine PG. The role of performance-avoidance goals and worry in mediating the relationship between stereotype threat and performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;45: 180–185. 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.005 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 113. Schwarz N. Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers. Am Psychol. 1999;54: 93–105. 10.1037//0003-066X.54.2.93 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 114. Nisbett RE, Wilson T. Telling more than we know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol Rev. 1977;84: 231–260. 10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 115. Mandler G. Consciousness: Respectable, useful and probably necessary In: Solso R, editor. Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2004. pp. 1–27. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 116. Miller GA. Psychology: The science of mental life Oxford: Harper & Row; 1962. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 117. Wheeler SC, Jarvis WBG, Petty RE. Think unto others: The self-destructive impact of negative racial stereotypes. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2001;37: 173–180. 10.1006/jesp.2000.1448 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 118. Wegner DM. The illusion of conscious will Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 119. Kiefer AK, Sekaquaptewa D. Implicit stereotypes and women’s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes influence women’s susceptibility to stereotype threat. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2006;43: 825–832. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 120. Townsend SM, Major B, Gangi CE, Mendes WB. From “in the air” to “under the skin”: Cortisol responses to social identity threat. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2011;37: 151–164. 10.1177/0146167210392384 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 121. Osborne JW. Gender, stereotype threat, and anxiety: Psychophysiological and cognitive evidence. J Res Educ Psychol. 2006:8; 109–138. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 122. Steele CM, Davies PG. Stereotype threat and employment testing: A commentary. Hum Perform. 2003;16: 311–326. 10.1207/S15327043HUP1603_7 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data availability statement.

  • View on publisher site
  • PDF (504.4 KB)
  • Collections

Similar articles

Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.

  • Download .nbib .nbib
  • Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

Add to Collections

IMAGES

  1. Stereotypes in the media

    stereotypes in research paper

  2. Stereotypes in the media

    stereotypes in research paper

  3. ⇉Stereotypes Research Paper Men in todays Essay Example

    stereotypes in research paper

  4. Formation and Implications of Gender Stereotypes Research Paper

    stereotypes in research paper

  5. White Female Stereotypes in Media

    stereotypes in research paper

  6. Research Paper 3

    stereotypes in research paper