Try Sketchy for Free

Free Trial. No commitment. No credit card required.

🏗️🚧 Sketchy will be down to level up! SCHEDULED DOWN TIME: 6/20 | 4AM TO 7AM PDT 🚧🏗️

Theories of Language and Language Acquisition

Psychology & Sociology

Language acquisition is the process by which children gain the ability to speak their native language, typically occurring before the age of five. The journey starts with infants engaging in babbling at around nine to 12 months. From around 12 to 18 months, they start using coherent words, followed by the naming explosion at around 18 to 20 months. By two or three years, children are able to form complete sentences, and by age five, they have more or less mastered their native language.

There are different theories that attempt to explain how language is acquired. The nativist theory , developed by Noam Chomsky, states that the capacity to learn language is innate, with a language acquisition device in the human brain. The learning theory or behaviorist theory , attributed to B.F. Skinner, proposes that language is acquired through operant conditioning. Lastly, the social interactionist theory , introduced by Vygotsky, suggests that language develops out of an innate desire to communicate and is picked up through social interactions. Additionally, there are theories that debate the relationship between language and cognition: linguistic universalism and the Whorfian hypothesis (linguistic relativity) .

Lesson Outline

<ul> <li>Introduction</li> <ul> <li>Language acquisition overview</li> </ul> <li>Language acquisition stages</li> <ul> <li>Babbling (9-12 months)</li> <li>First words stage (12-18 months)</li> <li>Two words stage: Naming explosion (18-20 months)</li> <li>Telegraphic stage: Stringing words together (2.5 years)</li> <li>Beyond telegraphic (3-6 years)</li> </ul> <li>Theories of language acquisition</li> <ul> <li>Nativist theory: Noam Chomsky</li> <ul> <li>Language Acquisition Device (LAD)</li> <li>Critical period of language acquisition</li> </ul> <li>Learning theory: B.F. Skinner</li> <ul> <li>Operant conditioning approach</li> <li>Reinforcement of correct language sounds</li> </ul> <li>Social interactionist theory: Vygotsky</li> <ul> <li>Innate desire to communicate</li> <li>Language acquisition through social interactions</li> </ul> </ul> <li>Language and cognition</li> <ul> <li>Linguistic universalism</li> <ul> <li>Thought precedes language</li> <li>Language created based on thought</li> </ul> <li>Linguistic relativity (Whorfian hypothesis)</li> <ul> <li>Language dictates thought</li> <li>Language influences perception of the world</li> </ul> </ul> </ul>

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Don't stop here!

Get access to 51 more Psychology & Sociology lessons & 8 more full MCAT courses with one subscription!

What are the main theories of language acquisition and their key principles?

There are three main theories of language acquisition: Nativist theory, Learning theory, and Social Interactionist theory. Nativist theory posits that humans are born with an innate capacity for language and a language acquisition device (LAD) in the brain. Learning theory suggests that language is acquired through a combination of imitation, reinforcement, and conditioning. Social Interactionist theory emphasizes the importance of social interaction and context in language learning.

How does the Nativist theory explain the process of language acquisition?

The Nativist theory, proposed by Noam Chomsky, asserts that humans possess an inborn capacity for language learning. According to this theory, humans are born with a language acquisition device (LAD) in their brains, which enables them to acquire and learn languages naturally. This LAD contains universal grammar principles that are innate and common to all human languages. During the critical period of language development, children are exposed to their native language, and the LAD helps them recognize and learn the language's specific grammar rules.

What is the role of social interaction in the Social Interactionist theory of language acquisition?

The Social Interactionist theory emphasizes the importance of social interaction and context in the process of language acquisition. According to this theory, children learn language primarily through the active engagement with their environment and social context, including interactions with caregivers and other people in their surroundings. These interactions provide opportunities for children to learn linguistic structures, vocabulary, and communicative functions through observation, imitation, and feedback. Social interaction is seen as a driving force that shapes language development and helps children to acquire linguistic skills and competences relevant to their culture and community.

What is the Whorfian hypothesis, and how does it relate to linguistic relativity?

The Whorfian hypothesis, also known as the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, is a concept in linguistic relativity that suggests the structure of a language can influence and shape the way its speakers think, perceive, and experience the world. This hypothesis posits that people who speak different languages may think differently because their languages shape their cognitive processes. The Whorfian hypothesis is commonly associated with two versions: a strong version, called linguistic determinism, which asserts that language determines thought, and a weak version, which claims that language influences, but does not determine, thought patterns.

What is the naming explosion, and when does it typically occur in a child's language development?

The naming explosion, also known as the vocabulary spurt, is a phase in a child's language development when they start to learn new words at a rapid pace. This phase typically occurs between the ages of 18 to 24 months, although the precise timing can vary among individual children. During the naming explosion, a child may learn several new words each day, rapidly expanding their vocabulary. This rapid increase in vocabulary acquisition is often attributed to a combination of cognitive development, increasing linguistic abilities, and the child's growing understanding of the symbolic nature of words and their connections to objects, actions, and concepts in the world around them.

MCAT® is a registered trademark of the Association of American Medical Colleges. The United States Medical Licensing Examination (USMLE®) is a joint program of the Federation of State Medical Boards (FSMB®) and National Board of Medical Examiners (NBME®). NAPLEX® is a registered trademark of the National Association of Boards of Pharmacy. PANCE© is a registered trademark of the National Commission on Certification of Physician Assistants. NCLEX® is a registered trademark and service mark of the National Council of State Boards of Nursing, Inc. None of the trademark holders are endorsed by nor affiliated with Sketchy or this website.

© 2013-2024 Sketchy Group LLC. All rights reserved

Back to Top

  • A-Z Publications

Annual Review of Anthropology

Volume 26, 1997, review article, linguistic relativity.

  • John A. Lucy 1
  • View Affiliations Hide Affiliations Affiliations: Committee on Human Development, The University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60637; email: [email protected]
  • Vol. 26:291-312 (Volume publication date October 1997) https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.26.1.291
  • © Annual Reviews

The linguistic relativity hypothesis, the proposal that the particular language we speak influences the way we think about reality, forms one part of the broader question of how language influences thought. Despite long-standing historical interest in the hypothesis, there is relatively little empirical research directly addressing it. Existing empirical approaches are classified into three types. 1. Structure-centered approaches begin with language differences and ask about their implications for thought. 2. Domain-centered approaches begin with experienced reality and ask how different languages encode it. 3. Behavior-centered approaches begin with some practical concern and seek an explanation in language. These approaches are compared, and recent methodological improvements highlighted. Despite empirical advances, a theoretical account needs to articulate exactly how languages interpret experiences and how those interpretations influence thought. This will entail integrating theory and data concerning both the general relation of language and thought and the shaping influence of specific discursive structures and practices.

Article metrics loading...

Full text loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article

Most Read This Month

Most cited most cited rss feed, ethnography in/of the world system: the emergence of multi-sited ethnography, the politics and poetics of infrastructure, migrant “illegality” and deportability in everyday life, poetics and performances as critical perspectives on language and social life, language and agency, refugees and exile: from "refugee studies" to the national order of things, parks and peoples: the social impact of protected areas, language ideology, think practically and look locally: language and gender as community-based practice, the local and the global: the anthropology of globalization and transnationalism.

Publication Date: 01 Oct 1997

Online Option

Sign in to access your institutional or personal subscription or get immediate access to your online copy - available in PDF and ePub formats

Complimentary 1-hour tutoring consultation Schedule Now

AAMC FL2 PS [Web]

Exam 2 P/S Solutions: Passage 1

1) This question involves knowing the four structures of the eye listed in the answer choices. We’ll have to judge which would be the most important when comparing the color perception of humans and baboons.  

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

  • Optic disc The optic disc is where the optic nerve connects to the retina, which you can see in the above image. It is the point of exit for ganglion cell axons leaving the eye. The blind spot you have in each eye corresponds to the optic disc.
  • Sclera The sclera is the white, outer layer of the eye. It’s mostly for protection and form. 
  • Fovea The fovea is the region in the center back of the eye that is responsible for acute vision. The fovea has a high density of cones. When you bring your gaze to an object to examine it intently in bright light, the eyes orient so that the object’s image falls on the fovea. This sounds like the most relevant function to color perception so far.
  • Lens The lens is a transparent biconvex structure in the eye that, along with the cornea, helps to refract light to be focused on the retina. Answer choice C is going to remain our best option. The fovea is related to color sensation, and that is where we’d want the most similarity in eye structure.

2) First part of our passage explained how humans abruptly switched from blue to green responses at a certain wavelength in the experiment. We perceive these colors categorically as either green or blue, while baboons do not. The plot for baboons will be different; we should see a continuum and a steady increase from green to blue. 

  • This answer choice incorrectly shows the percent of green responses do not change. The baboons are able to distinguish between the colors. This is an unlikely graph
  • This is just the reverse of Figure 1 in the passage and shows the colors being perceived categorically (either green or blue)
  • This is identical to the graph in the passage and also shows the colors being perceived categorically. We know that’s not the case in baboons.
  • This correctly shows a continuous pattern and a steady increase from green to blue. Answer choice D is going to be our best answer.

3) To answer this question, we’ll look through the four answer choices presented and go through the functions of each. We want the answer choice that’s related to viewing color, or vision in general.

  • occipital cortex. The occipital lobe controls vision, so right away we have a promising answer. We can look at our other answers and dive further in the functions if we need to get into further detail. 
  • temporal cortex. The temporal lobe controls hearing and some other speech functions. Answer choice A remains our best option because it controls vision.
  • somatosensory cortex. Like its name suggests, the somatosensory cortex processes somatic sensations. Our senses gather information, and the somatosensory cortex processes these sensations.
  • motor cortex . The motor cortex functions to control and execute voluntary movement. Out of all the answer choices listed, only the occipital cortex controls vision. Our best answer is going to be answer choice A. 

4) We can get this information from the passage.

I have highlighted what I feel is going to be necessary information from the passage. We have a positive banana chip when a correct response was produced. We have an aversive (strongly disliked) procedure when there are incorrect responses. 

  • Fixed ratio there are a set number of responses that must occur before the behavior is rewarded. This sounds like our experiment. The subject gets a banana chip after a correct response. 
  • Variable ratio the number of responses needed for a reward varies.
  • Fixed interval is when behavior is rewarded after a set amount of time.
  • Variable interval the subject gets the reinforcement based on varying and unpredictable amounts of time. The schedule that is used in the color-matching task is answer choice A: Fixed ratio.

Exam 2 P/S Solutions: Passage 2

5) First thing we can do here is bring up where the author talks about assimilation patterns in the passage.

We want to find variation in a set of answer choices and make sure we relate to this final paragraph in the passage. This is ultimately going to come down to knowing your vocabulary for the behavioral section.

  • Social status and roles . Social status refers to one’s standing in the community and his position in the social hierarchy. A role is a set of rules or norms that function as plans or blueprints to guide behavior within a particular society.
  • Social and cultural capital . Cultural capital is the accumulation of knowledge, behaviors, and skills that a person can tap into to demonstrate one’s cultural competence and social status. Social capital includes the advantages conferred by one’s social network, such as access to professional opportunities and insider knowledge.
  • Cultural values and norms . Values include culturally-defined standards that serve as broad guidelines for social living. Norms include rules and expectations by which a society guides the behavior of its members. Traditions and standards can vary by region, so there is variation regarding how immigrants adopt these local traditions and standards. There is geographic variation in cultural values and norms, so answer choice C is going to be our best answer.
  • Material and symbolic culture . Material culture refers to the relationship between artifacts and social relations; symbolic culture refers to the ideas, beliefs, values, or norms that shape a society.

6) The MCAT loves well-rounded students, and this question is an example of exactly that! We’re going to consider some historical factors for why there has been an increase in population of people aged 65 and older, and why there is a projected increase as well. 

  • The increased immigration rate since the 1950s . While there has been an increased immigration rate, the immigrants mentioned in the passage are mostly children, not individuals aged 65 or older.
  • The increased fertility rate after World War II . This is a very likely answer as it relates to the baby boomers, which is the generation of babies born after WWII. There was a big increase in fertility rate at this time, and these individuals are now over the age of 65.
  • The sexual revolution of the 1960s and 1970s . The sexual revolution is more a change in the way of thinking about how sex is perceived. This did not increase fertility rate like answer choice B. 
  • The relative deprivation of the Great Depression . Deprivation would likely have the opposite effect on the population. Fewer children would be born. Answer choice B is going to remain our best option.

7) First thing we’re going to do to answer this question is define conflict theory. Conflict theory is a way of studying society by focusing on inequality between different groups and sees social life as a competition and focuses on the distribution of resources and power between these groups. Conflict theory views change as abrupt and revolutionary; that change and revolution is the result of inequality. In the conflict perspective, change comes about through conflict between competing interests, not consensus or adaptation. The passage did talk about demographic change affecting the US healthcare delivery system, but we want to explain this from the perspective of a conflict theorist. 

  • This is more describing social constructionism. Social constructionism is a perspective that focuses on how knowledge and experiences are not real, but exist because individuals and society give them meaning. This collective meaning provides the reality we live in. Ideas, like health and disease for example, are defined and shaped over time by society, and therefore subject to different cultural norms.
  • This is more functionalism. Functionalism promotes solidarity and stability in a community but cannot account for the individual or any social changes
  • The focus on social support here might be a symbolic interactionism perspective. Symbolic Interactionism focuses on small scale perspectives with small interactions between individuals. It explains how individuals act in society and can be expanded to look at the interactions of larger social groups to explain social change
  • A conflict theorist would focus on a discrepancy in political power base on generational status. They would focus on the allocation of resources. Answer choice D is going to be our best option here. 

8) This is going to be a content question that’s tangentially related to the passage. We can define the four terms given as answer choices and pick the one that is most likely to increase as the US population ages.

  • the dependency ratio. This is a mathematical comparison of the number of people aged less than 14 and above 65 who are not part of the labor force, compared to those aged 15-64 who are part of the workforce. As we have a larger percentage of the population over 65 years of age and high birth rates, this ratio will increase. 
  • the social gradient in health. This refers to how wealthy people are found to live longer on average than middle-class people, and middle-class people live longer than poor people.
  • the life course perspective. Life-course theory views aging as changes in social, psychological and biological processes with time. Answer choice A is more obviously going to change with an aging US population. 
  • the intersectionality of medicine. All individuals have multiple aspects of identity, and simultaneously experience some privileges due to their socially valued identity statuses and disadvantages due to their devalued identity statuses. This is not going to increase with increased age.

Exam 2 P/S Solutions: Questions 9-12

9) The theory of linguistic relativity states that the structure of a language influences the way its speakers conceptualize the world. The canonical example of studying linguistic relativity is in the area of color naming. Sapir and Whorf would believe that people whose languages partition the color spectrum along different lines perceive colors in a different way.

  • All languages have a word for “up” and a word for “down.” While this may or may not be a true statement, we’re supporting the linguistic relativity hypothesis here, which doesn’t happen with answer choice A. This would be support for linguistic universalism. 
  • Humans are better at learning words for primary colors than for secondary colors. Humans can learn the words just fine, it’s a matter of actually defining these colors and names. If you know three colors for three shades of green, you’re able to distinguish and point out those shades of green when you see them. Just because green is a secondary color does not mean we’re any worse at learning words for green.
  • Some languages do not have words for “right” and “left.” Reasoning here is similar to answer choice A. While this may or may not be a true statement, we’re supporting the linguistic relativity hypothesis here. The structure of these languages influences how speakers conceptualize the world. 
  • Humans are better at distinguishing colors for which their language has a name . This is the classic example for linguistic relativity hypothesis. I mentioned this earlier when going through answer choice B. If you know three colors for three shades of green, you’re able to distinguish and point out those shades of green when you see them. If you’ve never heard of the name or word for something, it’s difficult to comprehend. Answer choice D is going to be our best answer. 

10) First thing we’re going to do here is define a source-monitoring error. A source-monitoring error is a memory error in which the source of a memory is incorrectly attributed to some specific recollected experience. For example, the fictitious names are going to be fresh in the participants’ minds, so they might incorrectly believe they remember the name because it is a famous person they’ve heard of. There’s no real confidence in how the name was learned: either through hearing about a real famous person, or from seeing the fictitious name the previous day. While they might recall some of the old fictitious names as famous, that likely doesn’t mean they will forget the celebrity names as famous. 

204FB_80188A.gif

Answer choice A reflects these observations. The individual will still recognize the celebrity names as famous people. They will incorrectly recognize some of the old fictitious names from the previous day as famous people because they do not recall how the name was learned. And lastly, there will be very few new fictitious names that the participants will recognize as a famous person. Answer choice A is our best answer.

To see the remaining Advanced Solutions, please download our Chrome Extension by clicking here . Once downloaded, while viewing the official AAMC products in ‘review’ mode, the Jack Westin AAMC Chrome Extension will overlay our Advanced Solutions to your screen.

This tutorial video will walk you through everything you need to know about using the Jack Westin Chrome Extension.

Please email us at [email protected] if you need assistance with using this free tool.

Your Notifications Live Here

{{ notification.creator.name }} Spark

{{ announcement.creator.name }}

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Trial Session Enrollment

Live trial session waiting list.

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Next Trial Session:

{{ nextFTS.remaining.months }} {{ nextFTS.remaining.months > 1 ? 'months' : 'month' }} {{ nextFTS.remaining.days }} {{ nextFTS.remaining.days > 1 ? 'days' : 'day' }} {{ nextFTS.remaining.days === 0 ? 'Starts Today' : 'remaining' }} Starts Today

Recorded Trial Session

This is a recorded trial for students who missed the last live session.

Waiting List Details:

Due to high demand and limited spots there is a waiting list. You will be notified when your spot in the Trial Session is available.

  • Learn Basic Strategy for CARS
  • Full Jack Westin Experience
  • Interactive Online Classroom
  • Emphasis on Timing

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Next Trial:

Free Trial Session Enrollment

Daily mcat cars practice.

New MCAT CARS passage every morning.

You are subscribed.

{{ nextFTS.remaining.months }} {{ nextFTS.remaining.months > 1 ? 'months' : 'month' }} {{ nextFTS.remaining.days }} {{ nextFTS.remaining.days > 1 ? 'days' : 'day' }} remaining Starts Today

Welcome Back!

Please sign in to continue..

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Please sign up to continue.

{{ detailingplan.name }}.

  • {{ feature }}
  • Search Menu

Sign in through your institution

  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical Numismatics
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Social History
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Language Acquisition
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Media
  • Music and Culture
  • Music and Religion
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Society
  • Law and Politics
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Legal System - Costs and Funding
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Restitution
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Medical Oncology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Medical Ethics
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business Ethics
  • Business History
  • Business Strategy
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and Government
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Social Issues in Business and Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic History
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Social Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Sustainability
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • Ethnic Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Political Theory
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Politics and Law
  • Politics of Development
  • Public Policy
  • Public Administration
  • Qualitative Political Methodology
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Disability Studies
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics

  • < Previous chapter
  • Next chapter >

38 Cognitive Linguistics and Linguistic Relativity

Eric Pederson (PhD 1991) is associate professor of Linguistics at the University of Oregon. The overarching theme of his research is the relationship between language and conceptual processes. He was a student at the University of California, Berkeley, working within Cognitive Linguistics with George Lakoff, Dan Slobin, Eve Sweetser, and Leonard Talmy since 1980. He joined the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics in 1991 until 1997, where he began working on issues more specific to linguistic relativity. Relevant publications include “Geographic and Manipulable Space in Two Tamil Linguistic Systems” (1993); “Language as Context, Language as Means: Spatial Cognition and Habitual Language use” (1995); “Semantic Typology and Spatial Conceptualization” (with Eve Danziger, Stephen Levinson, Sotaro Kita, Gunter Senft, and David Wilkins, 1998); “Through the Looking Glass: Literacy, Writing Systems and Mirror Image Discrimination” (with Eve Danziger, 1998); and “Mirror-Image Discrimination among Nonliterate, Monoliterate, and Biliterate Tamil Speakers” (2003). In addition to linguistic relativity, his general interests include semantic typology, field/descriptive linguistics (South India), and the representation of events. Eric Pederson can be reached at [email protected].

  • Published: 18 September 2012
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

Linguistic relativity (also known as the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis) is a general cover term for the conjunction of two basic notions. The first notion is that languages are relative, that is, that they vary in their expression of concepts in noteworthy ways. The second notion is that the linguistic expression of concepts has some degree of influence over conceptualization in cognitive domains, which need not necessarily be linguistically mediated. This article explores the treatment of linguistic relativity within works generally representative of cognitive linguistics and presents a survey of classic and more modern (pre- and post-1980s) research within linguistics, anthropology, and psychology. First, it provides a brief overview of the history of linguistic relativity theorizing from Wilhelm von Humboldt through to Benjamin Whorf. It then discusses the role of literacy to cognitive and cultural development, folk classification, and formulations of linguistic relativity.

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Our books are available by subscription or purchase to libraries and institutions.

Month: Total Views:
October 2022 11
November 2022 6
December 2022 8
January 2023 7
February 2023 13
March 2023 10
April 2023 6
May 2023 5
June 2023 8
July 2023 6
August 2023 14
September 2023 14
October 2023 5
November 2023 5
December 2023 18
January 2024 16
February 2024 1
March 2024 9
April 2024 22
May 2024 6
June 2024 10
July 2024 9
August 2024 3
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Toward a script relativity hypothesis: focused research agenda for psycholinguistic experiments in the science of reading

  • Open access
  • Published: 30 May 2022
  • Volume 6 , pages 97–117, ( 2022 )

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

  • Hye K. Pae   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-6181-775X 1  

3384 Accesses

4 Citations

4 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

The purpose of this paper is to extend the linguistic relativity hypothesis (i.e., the language we speak affects the way we think) to a script relativity hypothesis (i.e., the script in which we read influences our thought). Based on the rich body of knowledge in the science of reading that shows the effects of literacy on our cognitive processes, the foundation, rationale, and converging evidence of script relativity are discussed. The tenable notion of script relativity is anchored in previous research into the connection between language and thought as well as a causal relationship from language to cognition. Further discussed is the application of linguistic relativity to reading in both first and second languages to elucidate the reading-to-cognition link and how reading affects our attention, perception, and thought. Focused research for script relativity is suggested in the areas of the operating principle of script (alphabetic vs. morphosyllabic), reading directionality (left-to-right vs. right-to-left), word configurations (linearity vs. block), literacy experience (literates vs. illiterates), and interword spaces (presence vs. absence of interword spaces). The article ends with further recommendations and future directions. It is suggested that linguistic and cultural effects on cognition be controlled in future studies to disentangle the true effects of script.

Similar content being viewed by others

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Script relativity hypothesis: evidence from reading with different spatial layouts and varied lexical tone

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

The Idea, Practice and Power of Reading

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Introduction to Bilingual Research Methods

Explore related subjects.

  • Artificial Intelligence

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

Introduction

In human history, the invention of written signs dramatically changed not only the trajectory of civilization (Diamond, 1999 ), but also the way we think (Logan, 2004 ; Pae, 2020 ). Writing systems are only 5000 years old, which is a relatively short history compared to that of other inventions, such as fire, beer, musical instruments, and so on. Preliterate days comprise 99.9% of human history (Diamond, 1999 ). All remarkable advances made in the course of human civilization, such as the alphabet, printing press, the Internet, and digital text, to name a few, have emerged within the 0.1% of human history after writing systems were invented. Due to the significance of reading, notwithstanding the small segment of human history on an evolutionary scale, the aim of this paper is to elucidate the consequences of reading and the differential effects of the scripts in which we read on our thinking (action of reasoning) and thought patterns (product of thinking). The consequences of reading are explained through a lens of a script relativity hypothesis (i.e., the script in which we read influences our thinking and cognition).

At the core of reading is the script as multifaceted written codes. The term script refers to the graphophonic form of written language (Sampson, 2015 ). A script consists of a set of principles by which arbitrary signs are combined into linguistic units or words, which is in line with Weingarten’s ( 2011 ) definition of the script as a “set of graphic signs with prototypical forms and prototypical linguistic functions” (p. 16). A writing system refers to “the principles reflected in the fundamental writing-language relationships” (Perfetti & Liu, 2005 , p. 194). Relatedly, an orthography is defined as a physical form or “graphic format in which writing is represented” (Joshi & Aaron, 2006 , p. xiii).

Building upon the linguistic relativity hypothesis (Carroll, 1956 ) and existing studies (Ben-Yehudah et al., 2019 ; Cook et al., 2006 ; Dolscheid et al., 2013 ; Sato & Athanasopoulos, 2018 ), the script relativity hypothesis is predicated on the premise that the script being read affects the reader’s perception and thinking above and beyond linguistic effects. Specifically, the script relativity hypothesis refers to the tenable notion that the graphic forms and extralinguistic characteristics of writing systems—such as letter shapes (e.g., ascenders, descenders, dots, and curves as well as geometric- or angular-shape graphs in Korean Hangul), letter/graph configurations (Roman letters vs. non-Roman graphs), syllable or character writing (linearity vs. block), diacritics or circumflexes (signs written above, below, or next to a letter to indicate lexical stress, contractions, or tone), spatial density (visual complexities or the degree of crowdedness within a character), syllable format (horizontal or vertical syllabic format as particularly found in Korean Hangul and some Chinese characters), text direction (horizontal text vs. vertical text), and interword spaces (presence or absence of spaces between words)—may affect cognition beyond linguistic features. Cognition refers to the mental action and processes of incoming information, including thinking, thought, perception, recognition, conception, reasoning, and discriminant skills (e.g., visual discrimination). Script relativity can lend an additional avenue of research by disentangling the true effects of script for both first language (L1) and second language (L2) studies in the science of reading. To this end, we draw upon the linguistic relativity hypothesis due to the interlocking relationship between spoken language and written language as well as the close connection between language and cognition. It is impossible to discuss the nature of reading without discussing spoken language because a writing system encodes spoken language (Perfetti & Liu, 2005 ). As the most powerful cultural and cognitive tool, language serves as the medium of conceptual thinking and reasoning (Carruthers, 2002 ; Perszyk & Waxman, 2018 ).

In this article, the term reading refers to a process of extracting meaning from written symbols by drawing on linguistic and cognitive abilities. It is used in a narrow sense excluding context, affect, metacognition, and discourse because confounding variables associated with these factors are difficult to control for in a study and because reading in the narrow sense provides sufficient evidence for the script-thought nexus. Such a bottom-up approach is also consistent with the reading systems framework (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014 ), which places word reading at the center of the processes of word-to-text integration. The significance of the script relativity hypothesis rests on the importance of visual processes in reading to facilitate integration and establish the reader’s mental model of the text. Script relativity serves as a framework that explicates the cognitive dynamics of reading and consolidates the effects of script specificity or a script’s unique characteristics (e.g., letter shape, text direction, and presence or absence of interword spaces) addressed in a multitude of studies in various writing systems, such as English, Chinese, Japanese, Korean, Thai, and Hindi/Davannagari (Akamatusu, 1999 ; Ben-Yehudah et al., 2019 ; Das et al., 2011 ; Sun et al., 2022 ; Winskel et al., 2009 ). Notably, the findings of these studies are at the confluence of linguistic effects and scriptal effects on cognition. For the script relativity hypothesis, script effects need to be separated from linguistic effects.

Following an account of the linguistic relativity hypothesis, script relativity is discussed below as an extension of linguistic relativity. Focused areas of research are then delineated in light of testing script relativity. The article concludes with a call for research into script effects on cognition using scripts other than Roman alphabets, such as abjads, abugidas, Chinese, Korean, and mixed scripts.

Beyond the linguistic relativity hypothesis

The linguistic relativity hypothesis (a.k.a., Whorfianism or Sapir–Whorf hypothesis) refers to a proposition that the language we speak affects the way we think (Carroll, 1956). Since Whorf’s claim that language could shape cognition emerged as a theory in the 1940s, heated debate over its plausibility and generalizability had continued in anthropology, psychology, linguistics, and philosophy until the late 1990s. Few ideas have evoked as much controversy and interest as the linguistic relativity hypothesis in such disciplines (Lucy, 1997 ). The claim that language shapes cognition has two versions, comprising linguistic determinism (i.e., strong version; language determines our cognition) and linguistic relativism (i.e., weak version; language affects our cognition), although Whorf did not claim as such. This classification was posthumously made by Roger Brown after Whorf’s premature death in 1941 (Danesi, 2021 ). Although the strong version of determinism has hardly gained acceptability, the weak version of relativism has continuously been supported by a multitude of studies in psychology and applied linguistics (Bylund & Athanasopoulos, 2017 ; Dolscheid et al., 2013 ; Imai & Mazuka, 2007 ; Roberson et al., 2000 ).

Language-on-cognition effects: linguistic relativity hypothesis

Carruthers ( 2002 ) notes that language involves a “distinct input–output module of the mind” (p. 658), while thought concerns “discrete, semantically-evaluable, causally-effective states, possessing component structure” (p. 658). His delineations suggest an instrumental role of language that deals with the “module of the mind,” which, in turn, links to “semantically-evaluable” language. From this view, language and thought are essentially interrelated.

To better explicate linguistic relativity, there are at least two ways to examine relations between language and cognition. One way is to look at causation. The central tenet of linguistic relativity lies in causality from language to cognition. For a thorough analysis to ascertain comparative plausibility, causation and reverse directionalit y as well as independence should be noted. Another way is to look at the flipped side of this inquiry; that is, whether thinking can be restructured by language or whether language can be restructured by thinking.

Causation, reverse directionality, and independence

At the kernel of the debate over linguistic relativity are causal relations, which involve whether language affects thinking or thinking affects language . The debate can be summarized in three modes of relationships between language and thought as follows: (1) a causal relation from language to thought, (2) a causal relation from thought to language, and (3) no relation between them. The first relationship (i.e., language affects thought ) directly accords with the linguistic relativity hypothesis. This hypothesis posits that all languages vary in terms of grammar and semantic categorizations and that the structure of a given language affects thinking and behavior (Carroll, 1956; Lucy, 1997 , 2016 ). Whorf adopted the term linguistic relativity analogous to the physical theory of relativity, noting that different linguistic backgrounds lead observers to different views and interpretations of the universe (Danesi, 2021 ). According to the linguistic relativity hypothesis, the habitual use of language shapes the speaker’s habitual thinking and behavior.

The linguistic relativity hypothesis has been challenged mostly by nativists, particularly Pinker ( 1994 , 2007 ), in the 1960s through the 1980s. Whorf’s work was also criticized after the publication of Berlin and Kay's ( 1969 ) study of basic color terms. Berlin and Kay ( 1969 ) reported that lexical labels for basic color terms followed universal principles and the order of universal properties in focal colors. They also asserted that the typological patterns of basic color terms were the product of cultural evolutionary processes. However, a series of subsequent studies have led to modifications of their claims due to the language-dependence categorical perceptions of color (Kay & Regier, 2006 ; Roberson et al., 2000 ).

As cognitive linguistics ushered in the late 1980s and the early 1990s, many research findings were in support of the linguistic relativity hypothesis (Fishman, 1982 ; Lakoff, 1987 ; Levinson, 2003 ; Lucy, 1997 ). Levinson ( 2003 ) notes “… the ways we speak—the kinds of concepts lexically or grammatically encoded in a specific language—are bound to have an effect on the ways we think” (p. 37). Linguistic influences on our perceptual and cognitive domains in different language groups have been addressed in a wide range of domains from color (Masharow & Fischer, 2006 ; Roberson et al., 2000 ), number (Miura et al., 1994 ), time (Bylund & Athanasopoulos, 2017 ; Everett, 2005 ), motion conceptualization (Flecken et al., 2015 ), and nonlinguistic classifications (Imai & Mazuka, 2007 ) to musical pitch (Dolscheid et al., 2013 ). The overall findings of various studies Footnote 1 point toward the endorsement of the weaker version of linguistic relativity, which is unfalsifiable to a great extent.

The second relationship (i.e., thinking affects language ) has received much less attention than the first relationship . Although this possibility was raised (e.g., Pinker, 1994 , 2007 ), no research has been carried out to investigate this relationship. The lack of research in this line might have to do with the difficulties of testing and the implausibility or unfalsifiability of this claim. Studies of infants or toddlers can shed light on the understanding of links between thinking and language, given the lack of studies in this respect. A review of studies on infants’ conceptual development demonstrates that language serves as a hidden medium that facilitates infants’ concept learning through words and object categories (Perszyk & Waxman, 2018 ). Infants’ knowledge of object categories seems to be a fundamental step for their cognitive development from the age of three months onwards. Considering the notion that words are an invitation to the formation and expansion of cognition, language is considered an antecedent to the mind’s action (i.e., thinking), especially for high-level thinking. From this view, the assertion that thinking affects language is a moot point.

The third view of independence between language and cognition (i.e., language has no relation with thought ) is hardly conceivable based on research into infants as well as a host of philosophical accounts on the relationship between language and concept formation (Carruthers, 2002 ; Gauker, 2011 ; Gliga, et al., 2009 ; Perszyk & Waxman, 2018 ). Although Gauker ( 2011 ) essentially disavows linguistic relativity, he not only advocates for the language-dependence account of the origin of concepts, but also claims that we think in language. However, Pinker ( 2007 ) alluded to the view of independence, as in “… people understand reality independently of the words used to describe it” (Pinker, 2007 , p. 124) and noted “[s]ince mental life goes on independently of particular languages, concepts of freedom and equality will be thinkable even if they are nameless” (Pinker, 1994 , p. 82). However, he has not provided evidence for that in part because he has never conducted empirical research to test linguistic relativity and in part because this claim is not easily falsifiable. Carruthers ( 2012 ) noted that “[i]t is unclear whether anyone has ever really endorsed the thesis of the independence of thought from language in its most extreme form” (p. 383). As opposed to Pinker’s view, Fodor ( 2001 ) argues that we still do not know how human mental processes work, by claiming that a computational theory of the mind explains only a fragment of the mind. In Fodor’s ( 2001 ) view, cognitive science has not yet embarked on its journey to explaining the mind. Hence, this line of research needs to continue.

Can thinking be restructured by language? versus Can language be restructured by thinking?

As hinted earlier, another way to look at the relationship is through questions of whether we can think without language or not. Although the answer to this conundrum can vary depending on how thinking and language are defined, the notion of interdependence between language and thinking needs to be critically examined, especially considering infants’ category learning through language, as reviewed in Perszyk and Waxman ( 2018 ). Given that cognitive functions are acquired through language, not only are thought and language closely related (Carruthers, 2002 , 2012 ; Gauker, 2011 ), but language is also a means to express specific conceptualization of thoughts or ideas.

Because it is not easy to determine causation between language and cognition, it would be useful to look at the flip side of the two causal relationships by focusing on the outcome of the causation. The first flipped question is whether thinking can be changed or restructured by language. Evidence for this question can be found in the bilingual mind. Research shows that bilinguals’ and monolinguals’ attention, problem-solving strategies, and visual discrimination skills are different from each other (see Cook, 2015 , 2016 for review). This can serve as evidence for the claim that the mode of thought is/can be restructured by the consequence of language use. The second flipped question is whether language can be changed by thinking. There is no evidence for the affirmative answer to this question. Hence, this view has not been accepted in the literature, although it can be a topic of further discussion. Table 1 summarizes philosophical, anthropological, and psycholinguistic accounts by means of questions, answers, evidence, and conclusions for the relationship and the flipped queries that have been discussed so far. It begins with a fundamental question of whether all humans think alike. Linguistic diversity and cognitive diversity have been well documented (see Lucy, 1997 ). Considering research findings of bilingual or multilingual studies (Cook, 2015 ; Das et al., 2011 ; Sato & Athanasopoulos, 2018 ; Sun et al., 2022 ; Vanek & Selinker, 2017 ), it is difficult to dismiss language effects on cognition.

Extending linguistic relativity to bilingualism or multilingualism

The linguistic relativity hypothesis has been incorporated into L2 research, as fluid translinguistic influences from L1 to L2 help us understand the cognitive functions of language. A bilingual is viewed as a “many-sided whole” (Cook, 2015 , p. 154) who possesses multicompetence and shows complex relationships between language and cognition. Cook’s ( 2016 ) view of multicompetence involves the total system that functions for all languages in the single, but whole, mind of a bilingual. Research on linguistic relativity needs to continue because it allows us to better understand the boundaries of human biological and cultural diversity through cataloging translinguistic cognitive differences (Casasanto, 2008 ). We can understand how thinking works and how we acquire new knowledge through language experience by looking at cognitive differences among different language speakers. In a similar vein, Bylund and Athanasopoulos ( 2014 ) propose a new research program that applies linguistic relativity to L2 studies by recommending methodological and theoretical requisites for that direction.

There are a number of studies addressing the interaction between L1 and L2 (Ben-Yehudah et al., 2019 ; Cook et al., 2006 ; Sato & Athanasopoulos, 2018 ). Cook et al. ( 2006 ) found that learning L2 English influenced Japanese bilinguals’ categorization of objects and substances (shape or material), which were different from English and Japanese monolinguals. These results indicated that learning an L2 categorization had a significant impact on bilinguals’ conceptual representations, which provided support for the tenet of the linguistic relativity hypothesis.

More recent bilingual research has shown findings that support linguistic relativity. In a bilingual study, French–English bilinguals’ grammatical gender perception exerted a robust effect on the bilinguals’ judgments, indicating that the retrieval of prior knowledge associated with required grammatical properties was automatic and affected perceptual judgments, which was independent of task requirements (Sato & Athanasopoulos, 2018 ). Vanek and Selinker ( 2017 ) reported that Chinese speakers’ nonverbal event categorization was influenced as a result of learning temporal references in linguistic expressions in L2 English. They found that L1-modulated preferences also affected nonverbal judgments. These studies underscore interactions among language, cognition, conceptualization, memory, and L2 learning (Athanasopoulos & Bylund, 2014 ; Cook, 2016 ; Vanek & Hendriks, 2015 ), especially supporting the claim that different languages are likely to facilitate different patterns of nonverbal behavior beyond linguistic influences (Athanasopoulos & Bylund, 2014 ).

Toward the script relativity hypothesis

If spoken language is viewed as the most profound reflection of thinking, written language can also be viewed as the most profound reflection of what we think. Reading is a cognitive and metalinguistic process encompassing multiple componential skills, which involves visual discrimination, graphemic and phonological processes, retrieval of pertinent information from the mental lexicon, working memory, executive control, and prior frames of reference. Importantly, none of these skills is specialized or hardwired for reading.

Since reading is a neurobiologically demanding endeavor, habitual reading can shape and reorganize our cognitive structures, neural circuitry, and the brain’s inner-workings (Castro-Caldas et al., 1998 ; Dehaene et al., 2015 ; Huettig & Mishra, 2014 ). Because we are not born to read, we need to deliberately learn to read. As a consequence of many years of effortful practice of reading, the brain becomes rewired and restructured for reading due to innate neuroplasticity. In the journey toward gaining automaticity of reading, our brains accommodate the demand of reading by “recycling” the brain’s networks and pathways to tailor existing brain circuitry to the reading brain. To address this, Dehaene et al. ( 2015 ) call reading a process of “neuronal recycling,” which means that the brain recycles innate neuronal circuits to be able to read. The neuronal recycling hypothesis postulates that the architecture of the brain is highly constrained, but some networks are rewired to form new neuronal networks to meet the demands that are required for reading. Das et al. ( 2011 ) have reported that simultaneous bilinguals’ reading in different orthographies yields different brain networks showing script-specific plasticity, which operates through adulthood. Recent brain imaging studies also support the brain’s accommodation and adaptation to the script being read, suggesting that reading has remolded the brain’s circuitry in a certain way over time (see Huettig & Mishra, 2014 ; Kim et al., 2017 ). In short, the reading brain works as an engine that drives our minds, in that reading has essentially rewired the human brain and changed the trajectory of human civilization and history by transforming the architecture of our thinking.

Reading-cognition nexus: Script relativity hypothesis

Although about 7000 different spoken languages exist on the globe, commonalities are found in the relationship between spoken language and its writing system as well as literacy; that is, each writing system represents its spoken language (Perfetti & Liu, 2005 ; Verhoeven & Perfetti, 2021 ). The fact that the writing system closely aligns with its spoken language Footnote 2 suggests that the notion of script relativity fundamentally relies on the tenet of linguistic relativity. However, acquiring spoken language and learning to read are essentially different from each other. The former is acquired naturally with three conditions of exposure, time, and interaction, due to our innate ability to learn a mother tongue, while the latter requires conscious effort with no endowed ability that is prewired for reading. Hence, it is natural to deduce that habitual reading in a particular script over time affects the reader’s perception, reasoning, and thinking, which are the consequences of reading.

As a cognitive mechanism, reading involves the integrated use of graphophonic, syntactic, and semantic cues provided in written text. The interdependent relationship among these cueing systems is depicted in Fig.  1 . The graphophonic code refers to the visual cues provided through the sound-symbol correspondence and mapping. The syntactic code relies on the structure and grammar of a given language. The semantic code allows the reader to make sense of a text through context-dependent and language-dependent cues. These three cueing systems serve as an interdependent cueing system for comprehension. The componential processes of reading include decoding, word identification, meaning retrieval, sentence parsing, inferencing, monitoring, and comprehension. These knowledge sources are utilized in both constrained and interactive ways (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014 ).

figure 1

Componential Cueing Systems and Word-to-Text Integration

Given that the goal of reading is comprehension, the initial step to comprehension is efficient decoding and word identification. This aligns with the gist of the reading systems framework (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014 ), which explains the processes of word-to-text integration by placing word identification and word knowledge at the center of the model. This model links the word identification system to the comprehension system through the lexicon, which further explains the integration of word meaning into the reader’s mental model of the text. The processes of form-meaning mapping are central to reading, which in turn becomes a pressure point for reading comprehension. The integration processes further encompass orthographic knowledge, linguistic knowledge, and general knowledge (knowledge about the world, including knowledge of text forms such as text genres; Perfetti & Stafura, 2014 ). Lexical and sentence processing is considered to be different across scripts due to the specificity of a script.

With the influence of each script’s specificity, the automaticity of reading we acquire in childhood is likely to affect and shape our subsequent cognitive functioning and filter visual input, which yields a solid reading-cognition connection. The reading-cognition nexus varies according to the script being read. The reading-cognition nexus implicates a cognitive framework comprising visual perception (the process or awareness of visual information by means of semiotic and writing systems), reasoning (the process of understanding and forming inferences from premises expressed in text), memory (the mental capacity of recalling and recognizing information via writing systems), conception (an idea of reality shaped by the semiotic and writing systems acquired in childhood), and worldview (a comprehensive viewpoint of the individual’s interactions with the outer world).

Reading research agendas to test script relativity

Although the script relativity hypothesis draws on the linguistic relativity hypothesis, it is important to differentiate script effects from linguistic effects on the cognitive mechanism to identify true scriptal effects. A multitude of studies have investigated reading in various writing systems and its cognitive effects. However, existing studies have not controlled for linguistic effects in analysis. More systematic research designed to control for confounding variables is warranted to disentangle the true effect of script.

The impact of script relativity on thinking and cognition can be demonstrated at both micro and macro levels. The micro-level impact of script relativity refers to scriptal or semiotic influences on the reader’s cognitive functions. The macro-level impact goes beyond the individual level to the extent of discourse, rhetoric, and cultural and societal realms. To maintain the scope of a focused discussion, given that the proposal of script relativity starts from the graphophonic cuing system and visual processing, the micro-level impact of script relativity, primarily focusing on the graphophonic codes and orthographic systems, is discussed below.

Although trans-scriptal influences have been found and discussed in a multitude of studies in reading science (Akamatusu, 1999 ; Ben-Yehudah et al., 2019 ; Pae & Lee, 2015 ; Sun et al., 2022 ), the interpretation of previous findings has not been directed toward script relativity. Hence, it is time to systematically consolidate them into script relativity, and further test its validity in order to advance the theory in the science of reading. Results of L2 studies are particularly of interest, as trans-linguistic and trans-scriptal transfer is a manifestation of subconscious use of native or dominant linguistic skills in the face of the additional demands of L2 performance. Perfetti ( 2020 ) acknowledges that the thrust of the script relativity hypothesis is to open up a new quest in the science of reading. This quest needs to fulfill certain necessary conditions, which are summarized in a later section.

Below are areas for research, which are not mutually exclusive, that could further advance our understanding of how the script being read as L1 or L2 shapes our attention, perception, and cognition at the micro-level above and beyond linguistic effects. These research agenda items possess the potential to exert anticipatory and consequential effects of habitual reading. The effects of the script being read are discussed with respect to various scriptal characteristics, including the operating scriptal principle, psycholinguistic grain size, script shape, linearity, text direction, and text influences. Although previous studies did not directly test script relativity nor controlled for linguistic effects, they are worth mentioning below.

The operating principle of script: analytic alphabet versus holistic logography

The alphabet is governed by the alphabetic principle, meaning that letters or graphs (i.e., the general term for the smallest unit of written language; Sampson, 2015 ) represent sounds rather than meaning and that a cluster of graphs is used to represent a syllable. The alphabetic principle is the underpinning rule of alphabetic writing systems, such as Roman alphabets and Korean Hangul. Although the grapheme-phoneme correspondence varies across alphabetic writing systems, the smallest unit of the sound is the phoneme. Based on the regularity in the grapheme-phoneme correspondence, orthographies are classified into deep orthographies (i.e., the letter-sound correspondence is irregular; e.g., English, French) and shallow orthographies (i.e., the letter-sound correspondence is regular and consistent; e.g., Finnish, Spanish, Italian, and Dutch).

In contrast, Chinese characters and Japanese multi-scripts do not subscribe to the alphabetic principle. The Chinese writing system is logographic or morphosyllabic such that a written symbol represents a word or morpheme, not a phoneme. The Japanese use multi-scripts, including Kana and Kanji. Kana are syllabaries comprising cursive Hiragana (ひらがな) and angular Katakana (カタカナ). Kanji (漢字) are Chinese-derived logograms. Additionally, Romaji (the use of letters of the Latin alphabet) and furigana (used for glosses) are also used. Notably, all these scripts represent syllables, not phonemes.

The operating principle of the writing system is likely to affect how readers process written words. Since alphabets rely on phonology for the representation of written signs, readers of alphabetic scripts tend to rely more on phonology than other components of the word. This explains why phonological awareness skills are a dominant predictor of efficient reading in alphabetic orthographies.

A handful of studies have investigated trans-scriptal influences (Akamatsu, 1999 ; Pae & Lee, 2015 ; Sun et al., 2022 ). Ben-Yehudah et al. ( 2019 ) showed Chinese-English bilinguals tended to rely on holistic lexical information in recognizing upright and inversed stimuli, whereas Korean-English bilinguals were more likely to rely on analytic, sublexical orthographic information.

There is more evidence showing that different scriptal characteristics yield different cognitive processes. For example, a review of behavioral and functional neuroimaging studies shows that learning the auditory-verbal language system is significantly modulated by reading alphabetic orthographies (Petersson et al., 2001 ). Reading skills in alphabetic orthographies facilitate the awareness of sublexical phonological structures, which yields accommodated language pathways in the brain to efficiently respond to script characteristics.

Since the way in which visual information is extracted from text affects both word recognition and comprehension, script characteristics are an essential factor influencing reading. In particular, research into Chinese text can offer valuable information for the understanding of visual span and the way readers parse characters into meaningful units (i.e., words), as Chinese text shows no space between words. Yan and colleagues ( 2020 ) examined the perceptual span of typically-developing Chinese third graders who read age-appropriate text and found that their visual span was one character leftward and two characters rightward from the fixation point. Results also showed that higher reading fluency was associated with wider visual spans. A study of eye movement patterns of native Chinese readers in reading text with and without word boundary spaces showed similar findings (Bai et al., 2008 ). Bai and colleagues ( 2008 ) used texts of four types of spacing, including typical text with no-space, text with irregular spaces between words, text with spaces resulting in nonwords, and text with a space after each character. Results showed that Chinese readers did not show differences in reading text with and without space between words. However, reading text with nonword spacing and each-character spacing yielded a significantly slower reading time than that of the other two conditions. The results indicated that words, regardless of whether word boundary was implicit or explicit, were the unit of reading in Chinese rather than individual characters. These findings support the claim that readers of each writing system adapt their own visual exploration strategy to the script being read (Dehaene et al., 2015 ). Attention to this area calls for further systematic research or comparative research addressing the operating system across scripts by controlling for linguistically related variants.

Processing unit: phonemes, subsyllabic units, or syllables

Reading is related to psycholinguistic grain sizes and processing units. The minimal unit of sound in script varies across languages from phonemes to syllables. The alphabet has the minimal sound unit at the phonemic level, while the minimal sound unit of the Chinese and Japanese writing systems represents syllables. Within alphabetic scripts, the dominant psycholinguistic grain size further varies as the processing unit. In English, a consonant preserves its sound value even without a vowel, which allows for a consonant string within a word. For example, the word strong can be segmented into the onset str and rime ong ; the onset str is divided into individual phoneme /s/ /t/ and /r/; the rime ong is divided into /o/ and /ŋ/. Research shows that English-speaking children show a tendency to segment a word into the onset-rime unit (Treiman et al., 1995 ). In this regard, the popularity of Dr. Seuss’ rhyme books for emergent readers in the U.S. is not coincidental.

However, the onset-rime primacy is not universally found in alphabetic script readers. One example is found in Korean readers. Korean Hangul is an alphabetic script, but is written in a syllabic block (e.g., 학생, not ㅎ ㅏ ㄱ ㅅ ㅐㅇ, /hɑk sæŋ/ meaning student ). Furthermore, the Korean language does not allow for consonant strings as in English because each vowel should glue with a consonant. In other words, consonants and vowels function complementarily to each other. An extreme case is found in the word strike , wherein the word is a one-syllable word in English, but it becomes five syllables when the sound of the word is written in Korean. Because each consonant should take a vowel as a combinatory rule, each consonant within the consonant string in the word strikes takes an epenthetic vowel, resulting in six syllables with the diphthong broken into two syllables (i.e., 스트라이크스 /su Footnote 3 / /tu/ /rɑ/ /i Footnote 4 / /ku/ /su/). The writing convention in Hangul appears to yield a different processing unit than that in English. In contrast to the dominant onset-rime segmentation, Korean readers show preference for segmenting in the body-coda unit (Yi, 1998 ). Since this tendency seems to carry over to L2 processing, the role of the L1 grain size in L2 reading also points toward script relativity.

McBride-Chang and colleagues ( 2004 ) reported that different levels of phonological awareness are involved in reading across different cultures (i.e., China, Hong Kong, and Canada). They suggested that “the Chinese language [might] promote syllable-level awareness in children” (p. 93) due to Chinese being a morphosyllabic language. This line of studies should continue to test script relativity. Although comparative studies on English and Chinese languages have been copiously carried out, juxtapositions between alphabetic scripts and Chinese morphosyllabary are still useful to understand underlying mechanisms involved in reading and its effects on our attention, perception, and cognition.

Beyond the contrast made for grain sizes and processing units, alphasyllabaries (a.k.a., abugidas or akshara scripts) primarily belonging to Brahmi-derived Indic scripts, such as Bengali, Hindi, Kannada, Tamil, Tibetan script, and Thai, have a special status on the spectrum of grain size and processing unit. Such alphasyllabaries simultaneously represent sound at both levels of phonemes and syllables (Nag & Snowling, 2010 ). The duality of syllabic and phonemic representations in these writing systems, which make the script being referred to as semi-syllabic and semi-alphabetic, tends to yield differential contributions to children’s reading skills. Nakamura and colleagues ( 2017 ) have found increasing contributions of syllabic awareness through Grade 5, but steadily decreasing contributions of phonemic awareness to Kannada and Telugu decoding over time.

Grain size is also related to orthographic depth, which indicates the extent to which an orthography deviates from the one-to-one letter-sound correspondence. Depending on the degree of one-to-one or one-to-many correspondences on a scale, shallow orthographies (e.g., Spanish, Italian, and Finnish) or deep orthographies (e.g., English) are differentiated. The effects of orthographic depth and processing units on readers’ thinking, thought patterns, and cognition are a reasonable candidate for testing script relativity effects especially for comparative research. Although orthographic depth and processing units are coalesced with linguistic characteristics, these variables can be investigated in contrast to other script-specific variables in a study.

Script shape: Roman script versus non-Roman script within the alphabet

linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

Because visual cues are involved in the first phase of reading, script shape can exert an effect on its processing. Although the iconic quality of script has not been directly investigated in word recognition and reading in general, this line of inquiry needs to be addressed in the science of reading. The Japanese writing system can be of particular interest in this line of research. Japanese text can have a mixture of scripts within one sentence, including Chinese-derived Kanji, Katakana, and Hiragana. Research into the effects of visually more or less complex scripts as well as mixed scripts should continue to explore mechanisms behind visual cuing systems at the semiotic level, which also has the potential to extend to script relativity.

Configuration: linearity vs. nonlinearity

Relatedly, the ordering of graphs in a linear versus nonlinear manner also has an implication for the differential patterns of word recognition and long-term effects on cognitive processes. Roman script has linear letter ordering. However, non-Roman scripts, such as East Asian scripts (Chinese, Japanese, and Korean) and South and Southeast Asian alphasyllabaries (Devanagari, Kannada, Sinhala, and Thai) have nonlinear (or multilinear) orderings wherein graphs are packaged within syllabic blocks.

In particular, alphasyllabaries or abugidas are represented in symbol blocks at the syllabic level, which are composed of distinct marks representing phonemic-level sounds. Since vowels and consonants are stacked within a symbol block, the alphasyllabic writing systems are nonlinear or multilinear. The vowel marks or diacritics are secondarily ligatured to the base consonant to the right, left, above, below or around a base consonant, which makes the script visuospatially complex. Such scriptal characteristics require additional visual processing on top of phonological skills for efficient reading to abstract the alphasyllabic principle in reading (Nag & Snowling, 2010 ). This is consistent with a finding that Hindi written in Devanagari showed more increased demands for visuospatial processing than in English (Kumar et al., 2010 ). Although a considerable body of knowledge has been established in reading nonlinear scripts, an attempt to understand how such scriptal features function in the script-to-cognition nexus has been lacking.

Text direction: left-to-right versus right-to-left or horizontal versus vertical orientation

Another way to examine script relativity is to look at the effect of text direction. Although most scripts are written in a left-to-right sequence, there are a few scripts that are written in a right-to-left fashion. The investigations of text directionality (i.e., right-to-left Arabic, Hebrew, Footnote 5 and Urdu vs. left-to-right alphabets) can offer particular insights into the consequences of habitual reading. Vaid and Singh ( 1989 ) examined the influence of reading habit on the perception of chimeric facial affect (a half of the face is smiling, while the other half is neutral) among four groups of left-to-right readers (Hindi), right-to-left readers (Arabic), bidirectional readers (Hindi-Urdu Footnote 6 ), and illiterate subjects. Results showed no handedness effect among the groups. Left-to-right Hindi readers’ preference for the left hemifield was more pronounced than those of the other groups. Friedrich and Elias ( 2016 ) also found the effect of habitual reading direction on aesthetic preferences by comparing native Hindi and Urdu readers. Given that these two groups share linguistic similarities and geographical and cultural characteristics, a comparison of their aesthetic preferences would “reduc[e] the potential influence of confounding cultural differences on aesthetic preference biases” (Friedrich & Elias, 2016 , p. 128). Their results showed that native Hindi readers demonstrated a strong preference for stimuli that had the same directionality, while Urdu readers did not show this bias. They indicated that scanning habits as well as neural and anatomical asymmetries in spatial attention mechanisms were likely to be developed by habitual reading direction. These findings were consistent with other studies (Castelain & Van der Henst, 2021 ; Padakannaya et al., 2002 ; Pae et al., 2021 ; Singh et al., 2000 ; Vaid & Singh, 1989 ) that found effects of text directionality on perceptual and performance asymmetries, arithmetic tasks, spatial scanning, and spatial reasoning.

The effect of text directionality on graphic representations was also addressed in a more comprehensive study. Tversky and colleagues ( 1991 ) investigated reading directionality in graphic productions in terms of trans-cultural and developmental trends. Speakers of English, Hebrew, and Arabic were asked to organize the graphic representations of spatial, temporal, quantitative, and preferential relations. Children indicated like , dislike , and favorite food by putting stickers on blank pages. Results showed the effects of text directionality on their graphic productions. English-speaking children tended to put stickers of their favorite food in left-to-right direction. In contrast, Arabic-speaking children’s tendency was skewed toward the right-to-left direction. Hebrew-speaking children’s preference was in-between. Footnote 7 The direction of top-down was found to be universal in that this tendency was found across all groups and ages (i.e., children and adults).

Eye movement studies provide another piece of evidence regarding the effects of text directionality. Research shows that visual span involved in reading is asymmetrical in identifying three or four letters to the left and seven or eight letters to the right of fixation, on average, totaling 10 or 12 letters per saccade. Pollatsek and colleagues ( 1981 ) examined native Israeli readers’ eye movements while reading Hebrew and English text. Their perceptual span was asymmetric to the left while reading Hebrew text. In contrast, their perceptual span was asymmetric to the right when reading English text. These findings indicate that the different patterns of attention are shown according to the script being read. This gives rise to script-specific effects of text directionality. This line of research should continue to examine and test script relativity, as reading direction seems to have an impact beyond optical scanning.

Regarding the effects of vertical orientation, more recent research related to Chinese reading has also shown reading direction effects on cognition. Based on the reasoning that readers would apply reading direction to object counting direction, Göbel ( 2015 ) investigated how English readers (left-to-right reading) from the U.K. and Cantonese readers (a mixture of left-to-right and top-to-bottom reading) from Hong Kong counted stimuli presented in the horizontal plane and found that reading direction influenced the way of counting direction even within the horizontal plane. Objects were presented in horizontal, vertical, and square arrays. For the horizontal array, both English and Cantonese readers tended to count objects from left to right. For the vertical array, English-speaking children tended to count from bottom to top, while the majority of Cantonese-speaking children and adults as well as English-speaking adults counted objects from top to bottom. For the square array, all groups except English-speaking children started to count from the top left position (Experiment 1). In Experiment 1, the participants were asked to count a square array of objects after reading left-to-right or top-to-bottom text. Although Cantonese-speaking adults tended to begin counting from left to right with no reading of vertical text, they tended to count in the top-to-bottom direction after reading vertical text. This finding indicated immediate as well as longstanding effects of reading direction.

A similar finding was also reported by Chen and Friedrich ( 2015 ). They inferred the effect of reading direction on spatial–temporal cognition and compared the performance on a temporal judgment task between Chinese and Taiwanese readers, given that horizontal text orientation is a national policy in China, while vertical texts are fairly common in Taiwan. Based on the participants’ self-report, Taiwanese participants’ ratings of vertical texts and horizontal left-to-right texts were 5.7 and 6.5, respectively, while those of Chinese were 2.7 and 6.9, respectively. This indicated lopsided text familiarity in the two reading directions across the two groups. For the Taiwanese participants, vertical bias was robust with the vertical presentation but not with the horizontal one. For the Chinese participants, vertical bias was inconsistent with the presentation direction. The findings of the study support the relationship between reading direction and readers’ performance on a space-implicated task. Chen and Friedrich ( 2015 ) concluded that reading direction had an impact on temporal thinking.

Vertical reading fluency was also found in experts of Scrabble, which is a board game in which players put letter tiles together to construct words either horizontally or vertically. The findings of van Hees and colleagues’ ( 2017 ) study suggest that Scrabble experts’ vertical fluency has to do with enhanced domain-specific working memory and flexible stimulus classification processes. The findings of the studies reviewed so far converge on the salient effects of text orientation on how readers perform nonverbal and verbal activities. The effect of reading direction particularly dovetails with the notion of script relativity. Further research in other scripts with a more script-focused design would also facilitate our understanding of reading direction effects on our cognition.

The effect of reading: readers versus nonreaders

Reading is an activity in which meaning is extracted from written signs. The ability to convert arbitrary symbols into sounds in the language and to interpret the information that written symbols represent is a complex cognitive process. Illiteracy refers to the inability to elicit meaning from written symbols. The difference in cognitive dimensions between literate and illiterate individuals can be a direct indicator that illustrates the consequence of reading. Given that reading requires conscious effort to acquire the automaticity of reading and that reading itself is a cognitive activity, habitual reading can affect the areas of cognitive functions and discrimination skills as well as rapid retrieval from the mental lexicon. Research shows the robust consequences of literacy that are demonstrated in the efficient access to and retrieval of phonological representations of rapid automatized object naming in a comparison among unschooled illiterate, ex-illiterate (those who learned to read in adulthood with no schooling in childhood), and schooled literate adults (Huettig et al., 2018 ).

Using fMRI, Dehaene et al. (2010, 2015 ) indicated that learning to read, even in adulthood, could change or refine the cortical organizations and networks in the brain. Castro-Caldas et al. ( 1998 ) reported that learning to read in childhood was likely to shape the functional organization of the adult brain, by comparing word and pseudoword repetition performance between literate and illiterate subjects. When the two groups repeated words, no difference was found in their brains. When pseudowords were repeated, however, the two groups showed different neural structures activated in their brains. Petersson and colleagues ( 2001 ) also reported similar findings of fMRI studies in a review of the literature on cognitive processing between literate and illiterate groups. Although fMRI data inherently indicate correlation rather than causation, the findings of these studies give rise to the notion that habitual reading modulates or modifies brain functioning and brain structures, and further the human mind (Huettig et al., 2018 ). In a similar line, Fernandes and colleagues ( 2021 ) study showed the significant effect of the written script on mirror-image discrimination among illiterate, Tamil literate, and Tamil-Latin-alphabet biliterate adults.

Despite the growing literacy rates in developed countries, Sub-Saharan Africa and South Asia still have considerable rates of illiteracy at 35% and 27%, respectively (Statista, 2019 ). It may not be easy to conduct research into comparisons of literate and illiterate subjects with respect to cognitive profiles. However, this topic is a useful candidate for research to test the script relativity hypothesis. Considering that the findings of previous research have shown the effects of reading, the next attempt should be differential script effects on readers’ cognition.

Other scriptal characteristics that exert robust effects

There are at least six other areas of research that can test the script relativity hypothesis. First, the difference in the way of combining graphs or orthotactics beyond linearity versus nonlinearity is a useful option to test script relativity. All Roman alphabetic scripts arrange graphs in a left-to-right linear sequence, while the Korean alphabet, Hangul, is written in blocks with specific orthotactic rules. Since the visual configuration of Hangul is closer to Chinese than to English, Korean readers’ performance on visual word recognition is typically placed in between Chinese and English-speaking subjects’ performance (e.g., accuracy and response time) on naming, recall tasks, or lexical decision tasks (Ben-Yehudah et al., 2019 ; Pae & Lee, 2015 ; Sun et al., 2022 ).

Second, the degree of arbitrariness of written symbols (e.g., arbitrary symbols for Roman alphabets vs. semi-arbitrary symbols for Chinese characters and Korean Hangul) can also be a topic of research for script relativity. Signs are largely arbitrary, as they lack natural connections between a sign and its sound or between a sign and its meaning. Since most written signs represent arbitrary sounds associated with their meanings, arbitrariness is one of the linguistic characteristics common for almost all languages. Although Chinese logographic characters represent meaning, the Chinese writing system is not completely free from arbitrariness. This is true especially for simplified characters to the degree that Chinese is not purely logographic. Due to this quality, the term morphosyllabary is used to refer to the Chinese writing system. However, what is clear is that Chinese and English have different degrees on the continuum of arbitrariness. Studies that address the different cline of arbitrariness have the potential to contribute to the discussion of script relativity.

Third, the level of graph complexity can be another way to address script relativity. Chang and colleagues ( 2016 ) identified the degree to which the visual complexity of an orthography affected the initial stage of grapheme learning, by examining multiple graphemic dimensions of 131 orthographies in five writing systems (i.e., alphabet, abjad, alphasyllabary, syllabary, and morphosyllabary). As the visual complexity of a script affects the reader’s perceptual learning of graphemic forms, the effects of scriptal complexities vary across scripts being read. In addition, the two Chinese scripts (i.e., traditional characters and simplified characters) and the Japanese mixed-scripts also offer a unique opportunity to investigate readers’ visuospatial navigation as well as the effect of script complexity on cognitive or nonverbal activities. Chang and Perfetti ( 2018 ) noted that reading more complex scripts (i.e., traditional characters that have the higher number of strokes) would require stronger visuo-spatial skills than reading less-complex scripts (i.e., simplified characters). Cross-nation studies of Chinese reading between Taiwanese (traditional characters) and Mainland Chinese (simplified characters) also showed a significant complexity effect on performance on a same-different perceptual judgment task and a pattern recognition task (Chang & Perfetti, 2018 ). Taiwanese readers showed higher accuracy and faster response times than Chinese readers. This suggests that the more complex the script being read is, the higher visual perceptual skills are required.

In a similar vein, an eye movement study, exploring the impact of character-complexity on Chinese reading and visual search, showed that fixation durations and skipping rates were modulated by character complexity for both English-speaking and Chinese-speaking participants (Li et al., 2019 ). Reading Japanese Kanji has a similar effect of visual complexity on reading. Tamaoka and Kiyama ( 2013 ) examined how visual complexity functioned in Kanji processing using simple (2–6 strokes), medium (8–12 strokes), and complex (14–20 strokes) Kanji words with high and low frequencies. The results of a lexical decision task and a naming task demonstrated that reading low-frequency Kanji words was negatively affected by visual complexity of the stimulus.

Fourth, the presence or absence of a space between words is also a good candidate for testing script relativity, because not all scripts use spaces to demarcate word boundaries. Some Asian scripts, such as Chinese, Japanese, and Thai, use a series of contiguous words in sentence with no interword spacing. This scriptal feature requires readers’ efficient lexical parsing within the sentence (compare this sentence for readability with “Thissciptalfeaturerequiresreaders’efficientlexicalparsingwithinthesentence”). The level of difficulty in reading non-space sentences is greater in linear orthographies (e.g., English) than in non-linear orthographies (e.g., Chinese, Japanese, Devanagari). Previous studies have been conducted in reading Chinese (Bai et al., 2008 ), Japanese (Sainio et al., 2007 ), Thai (Kasisopa et al., 2013 ), and English (Juhasz et al., 2005 ). Comparative studies using different scripts within a study (e.g., Winskel et al., 2009 ) are needed to test script relativity.

Next, the Japanese multi-scripts offer a unique opportunity for understanding script effects within the writing system, as they use a mixture of scripts consisting of morphosyllabic Kanji (used for content words) and phonosyllabic Kana (used for function words and proper nouns). In Sakuma and colleagues’ ( 1998 ) study, significant homophone effects were found among adult Japanese readers in reading Kanji words, using a semantic decision task. Results showed that both orthography and phonology played a role in the recognition of Kanji words, but the effect of phonology disappeared when the item was presented only for a brief duration. The results suggest that orthography was a primary source of meaning extraction for Kanji words. Koyama et al. ( 2008 ) found that phonological and orthographic skills played different roles in reading Kana and Kanji for Japanese children. Phonological awareness was a significant predictor of Kana reading skills but not Kanji skills, while orthographic awareness and short-term memory skills were significant predictors of skillful Kana and Kanji reading. These findings suggest that readers can navigate the visuo-spatial features of Kana and Kanji differently in the face of a mixture of Kanji and Kana scripts within the same sentence.

Last, scripts that have extralinguistic diacritics appearing to the right, left, above, below, within, or surrounding a base letter, including Semitic abjads (Arabic, Hebrew), abugidas (alphasyllabaries of South Asia), some European alphabets (e.g., German umlaut, the French acute accent, the Czech hacek, etc.) are useful resources to test script relativity due to their distinct scriptal features and characteristics. Due to the unique script characteristics of being both a phonologically well-specified shallow orthography with pointed Hebrew and a deep orthography with unpointed Hebrew that requires readers to unitize words and morphemes via consonantals, Share and Bar-On ( 2017 ) propose a triplex model of Hebrew reading development, including (1) lower-order, phonological (sublexical) sequential spelling-to-sound translation, (2) higher-order, string-level (lexical), lexico-morpho-orthographic processing, and (3) a supralexical contextual processing of pervasive homography of unpointed Hebrew. This proposal may be Hebrew-specific or specific to reading abjads, and it warrants further research on the applicability to other scripts.

Share ( 2021 ) laments that the science of reading has long shown Anglocentirc bias by focusing on English, which is a writing system outlier because of its extreme inconsistency in spelling-sound correspondence. Share ( 2021 ) lists 10 dimensions of orthography complexity as follows: spoken-written linguistic distance; multilinearity and nonlinearity; visual confusability and visual complexity; historical change: retention of historical spellings despite pronunciation change; spelling uniformity despite morphophonemic alternation; omission of phonological elements; allography; dual-purpose letters; ligaturing; and inventory size (see Share, 2021 for details). By addressing the script features discussed above in reading research and in testing script relativity, we can overcome obstacles, such as Anglocentrism and alphabetism, that impede optimal progress in the science of reading. Research on such scripts’ within-language and between-language effects on readers’ cognition and problem-solving strategies facilitates our understanding of scriptal effects in terms of script-universality and script-specificity because their psychological implications for the script-to-cognition link are still unknown.

Necessary conditions for the script relativity hypothesis

Given that the script relativity hypothesis has to do with unidirectional relations, certain criteria need to be met in order to pass the necessary conditions for unidirectionality, which is similar to causal relations. According to Hill ( 1965 ), there are nine criteria for causality: strength, consistency, specificity, temporality, biological gradient, plausibility, coherence, experimental evidence, and analogy. The first criterion strength has to do with an effect size or a sample-based estimate of relationships between variables, which can be gauged by the magnitude of experimental effects. The second one consistency can be characterized by reproducibility or repeated observations of the same phenomenon. Specificity indicates a single effect with no spurious variables involved in the strength and direction of a relationship. Temporality shows that the cause precedes the effect in time sequence, which means that a reverse time order is unacceptable. Biological gradient refers to an exposure–response relationship (a.k.a., a dose–response curve) in which the magnitude of the response is determined as a function of exposure to a stimulus. Plausibility shows a reasonable relation. Coherence indicates a systematic and logical connection, which means that a cause-effect interpretation is not in conflict with natural principles. Next, experimental evidence needs to be empirically obtained by conducting experiments including intervention and prevention programs. Finally, analogy represents similarities between the observed relationship and any other relationships. An additional criterion of conditionality (i.e., if the cause is removed from the equation, the effect should not be observed) can also be added to the criteria.

These criteria may be saddled with exceptions in the determination of causation. However, they are useful to make reasonable inference based on observations or research findings. Of these standards, an essential condition is temporality (i.e., the cause precedes the effect in time) for a verdict on causation. This condition is a useful basis for the discussion of script relativity. When this is applied to script relativity, reading is a temporal antecedent to the effect such as the rewired reading brain (Dehaene et al., 2015 ), bilingual mind (Cook et al., 2006 ), or cognitive changes. In other words, the cause is habitual reading in a particular script, while the effect is a particular mode of operation in attention, perception, and thinking that is displayed as a result of reading by a group of readers who share the script of a written language. This causal relationship from script to cognition is tenable as previous research has implicitly shown (as reviewed above), whereas the reverse directionality is implausible. Another way to look at this association is whether the mode of operation in attention, perception, and thinking can be restructured by reading or whether reading can be restructured by the mode of operation. Again, the former is tenable, while the latter is not. Although it is difficult to test script relativity, systematic research is needed.

Broader impact of script relativity

As the reading systems framework (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014 ) underscores the processes of word-to-text integration, quality processes at the word level positively contribute to text comprehension. At the stage of lexical processing, diverse graphophonic representations across scripts (e.g., alphabets, abjads, alphasyllabaries, syllabaries, or morphosyllabaries) are likely to yield the different modes of sound-symbol mapping, visual exploration, and integration. The mode of processing can diverge into script-universal and script-specific processes engaged in reading. Script-universal processing involves the nature of orthographic-phonological conversions, while script-specific processing concerns orthographic units that a particular script shows.

Lucy ( 1997 ) asserts that linguistic relativity not only relates to semiotic-level characteristics of the language (a micro-level relation between language and thought), but also to discourse-level characteristics (how patterns of language use in cultural context can affect thought at a macro-level of relations). Beyond what has been discussed so far at the micro level of script effects, script relativity extends to the macro level as well. Figure  2 depicts the flow of script effects on both micro and macro levels. Unraveling the patterns of information processing upon reading and cognitive functions engaged in reading is one way to better understand scriptal effects on our thinking and to advance the sciences of reading, psycholingustics, comparative linguistics, education, and second language studies.

figure 2

The Micro and Macro Influences of Script and Advances in Sciences

Although it is possible to tease out the impact of script on comprehension using advanced methods (e.g., fMRI, well-designed masked priming tasks, eye movement studies) and analyses (e.g., ANCOVA, mediation analyses, path analyses, cross-classified modeling), reading comprehension is too broad to disentangle the true effects of script. Many componential skills, such as prior knowledge, vocabulary, syntactic knowledge, working memory, reasoning, inference, and self-monitoring, along with individual differences, can inadvertently function as spurious factors. The componential skills are also involved as direct and indirect contributors to comprehension. It would be better to start with a manageable bottom-up approach. Therefore, the macro-level effects of script in relation to discourse, rhetorical patterns, and culture are beyond the scope of the present article. It is still an open question how script impacts macro-level functions, which calls for systematic research in the near future. The macro-level effect of script can shed light on the understanding of language users’ or learners’ profiles in the disciplines of social psychology, trans-cultural communication, sociolinguistics, and anthropology. Bernstein’s ( 1971 ) notion of elaborated code and restricted code can be addressed at the macro level. The elaborated code applies to situations where no shared understanding or knowledge is available among members of a social group. The restricted code works well for insiders who share prior knowledge and mutual understanding of a given topic.

Conclusion and future directions

In this paper, the linguistic relativity hypothesis has been extended to a script relativity hypothesis, and focused research areas are recommended to specifically test this proposal. As Casasanto ( 2008 ) encouraged researchers to continue to conduct investigation into linguistic relativity in order to explain trans-linguistic cognitive differences and the boundaries of human biological and cultural diversity, it is also suggested that researchers tackle script relativity in data-driven research by controlling for confounding variables in the course of causation. Linguistic relativity can have profound implications for the study of mental representations because crosslinguistic cognitive differences provide useful information about how thinking works in our minds and brains (Casasanto, 2008 ). This is directly applicable to script relativity. There has been a lack of research that is specifically designed to test script relativity in the body of literature, although there is a wealth of studies that have investigated the relationship between literacy and cognitive processes. Empirical evidence gleaned from previous studies points toward the notion that script relativity rises above and beyond being subsidiary to linguistic relativity because script influences go beyond linguistic influences on thinking.

The quantity, quality, and range of research paradigms on linguistic relativity have significantly increased and broadened over time, which has resulted in refined explanations of language-specific effects on cognition (Lucy, 2016 ). Bylund and Athanasopoulos ( 2014 ) also emphasized the usefulness of studies of linguistic relativity, as in “the fields of SLA and relativity research have a tremendous potential for cross-fertilization” (p. 978). The heterogeneity and dynamic nature of nonnative speakers’ learning can be better explained in trans-linguistic studies. The same goes for script relativity as well. Drawing upon the studies of linguistic relativity, it is time to recognize script effects on our thinking and cognition beyond linguistic relativity and to consolidate previous studies through the lens of script relativity. Research on trans-scriptal influences would be a way to initiate a systematic exploration of cognitive mechanisms behind L2 reading and script-dependent reading specificity.

Given that script relativity is a proposal that explains the script-to-cognition connection, this hypothesis can guide the formation of novel theories and models for the cognitive dynamics of reading. This is particularly feasible considering that the word identification system requires high-quality scriptal and linguistic information for efficient word-to-text integration and is the basis of bottom-up input for building a meaningful comprehension system (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014 ). Recently, Winskel ( 2022 ) has joined the discussion of script relativity through a critical review of the literature in light of scripts’ spatial layouts and varied lexical tones and also called for further systematic research.

The script relativity hypothesis has the potential to address the aforementioned necessary criteria for causation with advanced methodologies (e.g., fMR that shows brain activation upon certain script processing, eye tracking techniques that allow for tracking attention shifts with script manipulations, masked priming paradigms, and Stroop tasks) and analysis techniques (e.g., path analyses, mediation and/or moderation analyses, cross-classified modeling) in cognitive sciences, applied linguistics, and psychology. It is particularly encouraging to test script relativity in the writing systems of abjads (Arabic and Hebrew) and abugidas (alphasyllabaries of South Asia; Kannada, Telugu, Thai, Tibetan) because these writing systems offer unique opportunities for elucidating scriptal effects on our verbal and nonverbal performance. The focus on non-European scripts also aligns with the notion of overcoming the Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) Anglophone science of reading (Share, 2021 ).

In addition, studies of sign languages and braille literacy will expand the horizon of script relativity. This line of research has great potential to provide unique insights into a better understanding of the consequences of literacy on the brain, attention, perception, and thinking in the science of reading. This effort also has a broader impact on democracy, as in Morais’ ( 2018 ) argument of dynamic reciprocity between literacy and democracy, which impacts the individual’s mind and brain as well as the levels of global and human history.

In consideration of space constraints, an exhaustive review is not provided here. See Chapter 3 of Pae ( 2020 ) for a more detailed review.

The Korean writing system, Hangul, is a good example for the claim that a writing system aligns with its spoken language, as it was deliberately invented to be a writing system that was compatible with spoken Korean language by King Sejong in the fifteenth century as a way to combat illiteracy resulting from the incongruency between the Korean language and Chinese characters (see Pae, 2020 for more information).

Although this sound is not the exact sound in Korean, it is an approximate sound, which is the closest among the IPA phonetic symbols.

When this sound is written in Korean, a dummy consonant is written before the vowel sound /i/ (i.e., ㅇ + ㅣ = 이).

Vaid and Singh ( 1989 ) pointed out the inadequacy of using Hebrew in writing directionality research in that many individual letters in Hebrew are written from left-to-right when they are written in a non-cursive style. Arithmetic and musical notations are also written in a left-to-right fashion, which may weaken directionality effects due to the exposure of left-to-right directions.

Hindi and Urdu (a derivative of Arabic used by Muslims in India and Pakistan) are identical spoken common lexicons, phonology, and grammar, but are different in the direction of reading and writing; Hindi are written and read from left to right, while Urdu are written and read from right to left (Vaid & Singh, 1989 ).

Young Hebrew-speaking children are taught to write numbers and perform arithmetic operations from left to right and perform arithmetic operations as such, whereas Arabic-speaking children are taught to perform arithmetic operation from right to left (Tversky, Mass, & Winter, 1991).

Akamatsu, N. (1999). The effects of first language orthographic features on word recognition processing in English as a second language. Reading and Writing, 11 (4), 381–403.

Article   Google Scholar  

Araújo, S., Fernandes, T., & Huettig, F. (2018). Learning to read facilitates the retrieval of phonological representations in rapid automatized naming: Evidence from unschooled illiterate, ex-illiterate, and schooled literate adults. Developmental Science, 22 , e12783. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12783

Bai, X., Yan, G., Liversedge, S. P., Zang, C., & Rayner, K. (2008). Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34 , 1277–1287.

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Ben-Yehudah, G., Hirshorn, E. A., Simcox, T., Perfetti, C. A., & Fiez, J. A. (2019). Chinese-English bilinguals transfer L1 lexical reading procedures and holistic orthographic coding to L2 English. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50 , 136–148.

Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution . University of California Press.

Google Scholar  

Bernstein, B. (1971). Class, codes, and control: Theoretical studies towards a sociology of language (Vol. 1). Loutledge and Kegan.

Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2014). Linguistic relativity in SLA: Toward a new research program. Language Learning, 64 , 952–985.

Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2017). The Whorfian time warp: Representing duration through the language hourglass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146 (7), 911–916.

Carnes, R. L. (2014/1970). A perceptual model of the Whorfian thesis. et Cetera, 71 (3), 263-271

Carroll, J. B. (Ed.). (1956). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writing of Benjamin Lee Whorf . Cambridge: MIT Press.

Carruthers, P. (2002). The cognitive function of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25 , 657–725.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Carruthers, P. (2012). Language in cognition. In E. Margolis, R. Samuels, & S. P. Stitch (Eds.), The handbook of philosophy of cognitive science (pp. 361–401). Oxford University Press.

Casasanto, D. (2008). Who’s afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Language Learning, 58 , 63–79.

Castelain, T., & Van der Henst, J.-B. (2021). The influence of language on spatial reasoning: Reading habits modulate the formulation of conclusions and the integration of premises. Frontiers in Psychology, Article, 654266 , 1–12.

Castro-Caldas, A., Petersson, K. M., Reis, A., Stone-Elander, S., & Ingvar, M. (1998). The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain, 121 , 1053–1063.

Chang, L.-Y., & Perfetti, C. A. (2018). Visual factors in writing system variation: Measurement and implications for reading. Writing systems, reading processes, and cross-linguistic influences: Reflections from the Chinese, Japanese and Korean languages (pp. 49–71). John Benjamins.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Chang, L.-Y., Plaut, D. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (2016). Visual complexity in orthographic learning: Modeling learning across writing system variations. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20 , 64–85.

Chen, J.-Y., & Friedrich, M. (2015). The effect of immediate and lifetime experience of reading horizontal and vertical texts on Chinese speakers’ temporal orientation. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 15 , 1–12.

Cook, V. (2015). Discussing the language and thought of motion in second language speakers. Modern Language Journal, 99 (Supplement 2015), 154–164.

Cook, V. (2016). Where Is the Native Speaker Now? TESOL Quarterly, 50 , 186–198.

Cook, V., Bassetti, B., Kasai, C., Sasaki, M., & Takahashi, J. A. (2006). Do bilinguals have different concepts? The case of shape and material in Japanese L2 users of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10 , 137–152.

Danesi, M. (2021). Linguistic relativity today: Language, mind, society, and the foundations of linguistic anthropology . Routledge.

Book   Google Scholar  

Das, T., Padakannaya, P., Pugh, K. R., & Singh, N. C. (2011). Neuroimaging reveals dual routes to reading in simultaneous proficient readers of two orthographies. NeuroImage, 54 (2), 1476–1487.

Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2015). Illiterate to literate: Behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nature Review Neuroscience, 16 (4), 234–244.

Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., & Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330 , 1359–1364.

Diamond, J. (1999). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies . W. W. Norton & Company.

Dolscheid, S., Shayan, S., Majid, A., & Casasanto, D. (2013). The thickness of musical pitch: Psychophysical evidence for linguistic relativity. Psychological Science, 24 (5), 613–621.

Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology, 46 (4), 621–646.

Fernandes, T., Arunkumar, M., & Huettig, F. (2021). The role of the written script in shaping mirror-image discrimination: Evidence from illiterate, Tamil literate, and Tamil-Latin-alphabet bi-literate adults. Cognition, 206 , 104493.

Fishman, J. A. (1982). Whorfianism of the third kind: Ethnolinguistic diversity as a worldwide societal asset (The Whorfian Hypothesis: Varieties of validation, confirmation, and disconfirmation II). Language in Society, 11 (1), 1–14.

Flecken, M., Athanasopoulos, P., Kuipers, J. R., & Thierry, G. (2015). On the road to somewhere: Brain potentials reflect language effects on motion event perception. Cognition, 141 , 41–51.

Fodor, J. (2001). The mind doesn’t work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology . MIT Press.

Friedrich, T. E., & Elias, L. J. (2016). The write bias: The influence of native writing direction on aesthetic preference biases. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10 (2), 128–133.

Gauker, C. (2011). Words and images: An essay on the origin of ideas . University Press.

Gliga, T., Volein, A., & Gsibra, G. (2009). Verbal labels modulate perceptual object processing in 1-year-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 , 2781–2789.

Gnanadesikan, A. E. (2020). S1: The native script effect. In Y. Haralambous (Ed.), Grapholinguistics in the 21st century — 2020 . Proceedings , 4, 103–123.

Göbel, S. M. (2015). Up or down? Reading direction influences vertical counting direction in the horizontal plane—a cross-cultural comparison. Frontiers in Psychology, 6 Article, 228 , 1–12.

Hill, A. B. (1965). The environment and disease: Association or causation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 58 (5), 295–300.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Huettig, F., & Mishra, R. K. (2014). How literacy acquisition affects the illiterate mind—A critical examination of theories and evidence. Language and Linguistics Compass, 8(10) , 401–427.

Huettig, F., Kolinsky, R., & Lachmann, T. (2018). The culturally co-opted brain: How literacy affects the human mind. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33 (3), 275–277.

Imai, M., & Mazuka, R. (2007). Language-relative construal of individuation constrained by universal ontology: Revisiting language universals and linguistic relativity. Cognitive Science, 31 , 385–413.

Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of orthography and literacy . Lawrence Erlbaum Associate.

Juhasz, B. J., Inhoff, A. W., & Rayner, K. (2005). The role of interword spaces in the processing of English compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20 , 291–316.

Kasisopa, B., Reilly, R. G., Luksaneeyanawin, S., & Burnham, D. (2013). Eye movements while reading an unspaced writing system: The case of Thai. Vision Research, 86 , 71–80.

Kay, P., & Regier, T. (2006). Language, thought and color: Recent developments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10 (2), 51–54.

Kim, S. Y., Liu, L., & Cao, F. (2017). How does first langauge (L1) influence second language (L2) reading in the brain? Evidence from Korean-English and Chinese-English bilinguals. Brain & Language, 171 , 1–13.

Koyama, M. S., Hansen, P. C., & Stein, J. F. (2008). Logographic Kanji versus phonographic Kana in literacy acquisition. Learning, Skill Acquisition, Reading, and Dyslexia, 1145 (1), 41–55.

Kumar, U., Das, T., Bapi, R. S., Padakannaya, P., Joshi, R. M., & Singh, N. C. (2010). Reading different orthographies: an fMRI study of phrase reading in Hindi-English bilinguals. Reading and Writing, 23 , 239–255.

Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind . University of Chicago Press.

Levinson, S. C. (2003). Language and mind: Let’s get the issues straight! In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 25–46). MIT Press.

Li, L., Li, S., Xie, F., Chang, M., McGowan, V. A., Wang, J., & Paterson, K. B. (2019). Establishing a role for the visual complexity of linguistic stimuli in age-related reading difficulty: Evidence from eye movements during Chinese reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 81 , 2626–2634.

Lin, C. Y., Wang, M., & Singh, A. (2018). Introduction to script processing in Chinese and cognitive consequences for bilingual reading. Writing systems, reading processes, and cross-linguistic influences: Reflections from the Chinese, Japanese, and Korean languages (pp. 25–47). John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Logan, R. K. (2004). The alphabet effect : A media ecology understanding of the making of Western civilization . Hampton Press.

Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology, 26 , 291–312.

Lucy, J. A. (2016). Recent advances in the study of linguistic relativity in historical context: A critical assessment. Language Learning, 66 , 487–515.

Mashrow, M., & Fischer, M. (2006). Linguistic relativity: Does language help or hinder perception? Current Biology, 16 (8), R289–R291.

McBride-Chang, C., Bialystok, E., Chong, K. K., & Li, Y. (2004). Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89 (2), 93–111.

Miura, I. T., Okamoto, Y., Kim, C. C., Chang, C.-M., Steere, M., & Fayol, M. (1994). Comparisons of children’s cognitive representation of number: China, France, Japan, Korea, Sweden, and the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17 (3), 401–411.

Morais, J. (2018). Literacy and democracy. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33 (3), 351–372.

Nag, S., & Snowling, M. J. (2010). Cognitive profiles of poor readers of Kannada. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 25 (6), 657–676.

Nakamura, P. R., Joshi, R. M., & Ji, X. R. (2017). Investigating the asymmetrical roles of syllabic and phonemic awareness in Akshara processing. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51 (5), 499–506.

Padakannaya, P., Devi, M. L., Zaveria, B., Chengappa, S. K., & Vaid, J. (2002). Directional scanning effect and strength of reading habit in picture naming and recall. Brain and Cognition, 48 (2–3), 484–490.

Pae, H. K. (2020). Script effects as the hidden drive of the human mind, cognition, and culture . Springer.

Pae, H. K., Bae, S., & Yi, K. (2021). Horizontal orthography versus vertical orthography: The effects of writing direction and syllabic format on visual word recognition in Korean Hangul. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74 (3), 443–458.

Pae, H. K., & Lee, Y.-W. (2015). The Resolution of visual noise in word recognition. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44 (3), 337–358.

Perfetti, C. A. (2020). Foreword. In H. K. Pae (Ed.), Script effects as the hidden drive of the human mind, cognition, and culture (vii-viii). New York: Springer.

Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Orthography to phonology and meaning: Comparisons across and within writing systems. Reading and Writing, 18 , 193–210.

Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18 (1), 22–37.

Perszyk, D. R., & Waxman, S. R. (2018). Linking language and cognition in infancy. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 69 , 231–250.

Petersson, K. M., Reis, Al., & Ingvar, M. (2001). Cognitive processing in literate and illiterate subjects: A review of some recent behavioral and functional neuroimaging data. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42 , 251–267.

Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language . HarperCollins.

Pinker, S. (2007). The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature . Penguin.

Pollatsek, A., Bolozky, S., Well, A. D., & Rayner, K. (1981). Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli readers. Brain and Language, 14 (1), 174–180.

Roberson, D., Davies, I., & Davidoff, J. (2000). Color categories are not universal: Replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129 (3), 369–398.

Sainio, M., Hyönä, J., Bingushi, K., & Bertram, R. (2007). The role of interword spacing in reading Japanese: An eye movement study. Vision Research, 47 , 2575–2584.

Sakuma, N., Sasanuma, S., Tatsumi, I. F., & Masaki, S. (1998). Orthography and phonology in reading Japanese kanji words: Evidence from the semantic decision task with homophones. Memory and Cognition, 26 (1), 75–87.

Sampson, J. (2015). Writing systems (2nd ed.). Equinox.

Samuel, S., Cole, G., & Eacott, M. J. (2019). Grammatical gender and linguistic relativity: A systematic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26 , 1767–1786.

Sato, S., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2018). Grammatical gender affects gender perception: Evidence for the structural-feedback hypothesis. Cognition, 176 , 220–231.

Share, D. L. (2021). Is the science of reading just the science of reading English? Reading Research Quarterly, 56 (S1), S391–S402.

Share, D. L., & Bar-On, A. (2017). Learning to read a Semitic abjad: The triplex model of Hebrew reading development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51 (5), 444–453.

Singh, M., Vaid, J., & Sakhuja, T. (2000). Reading/writing vs. Handedness influences on line length estimation. Brain and Cognition, 43 , 398–402.

Stastista (2019). The illiteracy rate among all adults (over 15-year-old) in 2019, by world region. Retrieved January 25, 2021. https://www.statista.com/statistics/262886/illiteracy-rates-by-world-regions/

Sun, J., Pae, H. K., & Ai, H. (2022). The recognition of coordinative compound words by learners of Chinese as a foreign language: A mixed methods study. Foreign Language Annals, 54 , 923–951.

Tamaoka, K., & Kiyama, S. (2013). The effects of visual complexity for Japanese Kanji Processing with high and low frequencies. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 26 , 205–223.

Treiman, R., Fowler, C. A., Gross, J., Berch, D., & Weatherston, S. (1995). Syllable structure or word structure: Evidence for onset and rime units with disyllabic and trisyllabic stimuli. Journal of Memory and Language, 34 , 132–155.

Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S. K., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23 , 515–557.

Vaid, J., & Singh, M. (1989). Asymmetries in the perception of facial afface: Is there an influence of reading habits? Neurolopsychologia, 27 , 1277–1287.

van Hees, S., Seyffarth, S., Pexman, P. M., Cortese, F., & Protzner, A. B. (2017). An ERP investigation of vertical reading fluency in Scrabble experts. Brain Research, 1667 , 1–10.

Vanek, N., & Hendriks, H. (2015). Convergence of temporal reference frames in sequential bilinguals: event structuring unique to second language users. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (4), 753–768.

Vanek, N., & Selinker, L. (2017). Covariation between temporal interlanguage features and nonverbal event categorisation. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 55 (3), 223–243.

Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2021). Universals in learning to read across languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading . https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2021.1938575

Weingarten, R. (2011). Comparative Graphematics. Written Language & Literacy, 14 (1), 12–38.

Winskel, H. (2022). Script relativity hypothesis: evidence from reading with different spatial layouts and varied lexical tone. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal , 35 , 1323–1341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10225-7

Winskel, H., Radach, R., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2009). Eye movements when reading paced and unspaced Thai and English: A comparison of Thai-English bilinguals and English monolinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 61 , 339–351.

Yan, M., Li, H., Su, Y., Cao, Y., & Pan, J. (2020). The perceptual span and individual differences among Chinese children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 24(6), 520–530 .

Yi, K. (1998). The internal structure of Korean syllables: Rhyme or body? Korean Journal of Experimental and Cognitive Psychology, 10 , 67–83.

Download references

The author appreciates an Open Educational Resources Grant provided by the University of Cincinnati Press and CLIPS.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Literacy and Second Language Studies Program, School of Education, College of Education, Criminal Justice, and Human Services, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hye K. Pae .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interests.

The author declares that there is no conflict of interest associated with this article.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Pae, H.K. Toward a script relativity hypothesis: focused research agenda for psycholinguistic experiments in the science of reading. J Cult Cogn Sci 6 , 97–117 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-022-00103-1

Download citation

Received : 12 August 2021

Revised : 26 April 2022

Accepted : 30 April 2022

Published : 30 May 2022

Issue Date : September 2022

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-022-00103-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Linguistic relativity
  • Script relativity
  • Trans-scriptal transfer
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Get the Reddit app

The #1 social media platform for MCAT advice. The MCAT (Medical College Admission Test) is offered by the AAMC and is a required exam for admission to medical schools in the USA and Canada. /r/MCAT is a place for MCAT practice, questions, discussion, advice, social networking, news, study tips and more. Check out the sidebar for useful resources & intro guides. Post questions, jokes, memes, and discussions.

Can someone explain linguistic relativism?

Is it right to say that this

Also believes that cognition can influence language development without entirely controlling it.

By continuing, you agree to our User Agreement and acknowledge that you understand the Privacy Policy .

Enter the 6-digit code from your authenticator app

You’ve set up two-factor authentication for this account.

Enter a 6-digit backup code

Create your username and password.

Reddit is anonymous, so your username is what you’ll go by here. Choose wisely—because once you get a name, you can’t change it.

Reset your password

Enter your email address or username and we’ll send you a link to reset your password

Check your inbox

An email with a link to reset your password was sent to the email address associated with your account

Choose a Reddit account to continue

IMAGES

  1. AAMC FL p/s #9 Linguistic relativity hypothese vs. linguistic

    linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

  2. PPT

    linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

  3. PPT

    linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

  4. The Linguistic relativity hypothesis in Language, Diversity and Thought

    linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

  5. Linguistic Relativism vs. Determinism : r/Mcat

    linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

  6. AAMC Fl 1 P/S: 36 Isn't Linguistic relativity part of the Weak

    linguistic relativity hypothesis mcat

VIDEO

  1. MIT CSAIL Explains: Large Language Models: Part 1

  2. language in culture and culture in language

  3. Linguistic Relativity in the Workplace

  4. MCAT Math

  5. CAT 2006

  6. A whole new meaning for "linguistic relativity" #linguistics #immigrants #etymology #language

COMMENTS

  1. Influence Of Language On Cognition

    Influence of Language on Cognition. Topic: Language. Language and cognition tend to interact in a dual and cyclical relationship, a theory known overall as linguistic relativity. What one thinks becomes what one communicates and what one communicates can lead to new thoughts. Several different theories aim to discuss the relationship between ...

  2. AAMC FL p/s #9 Linguistic relativity hypothese vs. linguistic ...

    The MCAT (Medical College Admission Test) is offered by the AAMC and is a required exam for admission to medical schools in the USA and Canada. /r/MCAT is a place for MCAT practice, questions, discussion, advice, social networking, news, study tips and more. ... strong linguistic determinism is just strong linguistic relativity hypothesis? I am ...

  3. AAMC Fl2 p/s #9 : r/Mcat

    The MCAT (Medical College Admission Test) is offered by the AAMC and is a required exam for admission to medical schools in the USA and Canada. /r/MCAT is a place for MCAT practice, questions, discussion, advice, social networking, news, study tips and more. ... Which finding best supports the linguistic relativity hypothesis? A.All languages ...

  4. Sapir-Whorfian hypothesis vs linguistic relativity hypothesis : r/Mcat

    Linguistic relativity falls under the umbrella of the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, which includes linguistic relativity (language influences cognition) and the more extreme linguistic determination (language controls cognition). I think either term, Sapir-Whorf and linguistic relativity, can be used to say language influences cognition.

  5. Theories of Language and Language Acquisition

    The Whorfian hypothesis, also known as the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, is a concept in linguistic relativity that suggests the structure of a language can influence and shape the way its speakers think, perceive, and experience the world. This hypothesis posits that people who speak different languages may think differently because their languages ...

  6. Language and Cognition for the MCAT: Everything You Need to Know

    The Whorfian hypothesis suggests that our perception of the world is somewhat shaped by the language we use. For instance, repeated use of pronouns within certain languages (such as English) may promote a perception of the world that is centered around individuals, whereas the exclusion of pronouns in other languages (such as Japanese) may lead ...

  7. Linguistic Relativity

    The linguistic relativity hypothesis, the proposal that the particular language we speak influences the way we think about reality, forms one part of the broader question of how language influences thought. Despite long-standing historical interest in the hypothesis, there is relatively little empirical research directly addressing it. Existing empirical approaches are classified into three types.

  8. AAMC MCAT Practice Exam 2 Ps Solutions

    While this may or may not be a true statement, we're supporting the linguistic relativity hypothesis here. The structure of these languages influences how speakers conceptualize the world. Humans are better at distinguishing colors for which their language has a name. This is the classic example for linguistic relativity hypothesis.

  9. PDF Linguistic relativity

    thinking for speaking, linguistic relativity effects should disappear (Clark 2003; Papafragou et al. 2007; Landau et al. 2010). For example, Eve Clark predicted that if truly nonlinguistic tests of linguistic relativity could be devised, their results should differ dramatically from the results of thinking-for-speaking-driven experiments:

  10. PDF Chapter 1 Linguistic Relativity in Conceptual Metaphors

    The linguistic relativity hypothesis suggests that the categorical perception of color varies as language varies, as supported by the findings of cross-lin-guistic studies. For example, Roberson and his colleagues (e.g., Roberson & Davidoff, 2000; Roberson, Davies, & Davidoff, 2000) examined the relation-

  11. Cognitive Linguistics and Linguistic Relativity

    Linguistic relativity (also known as the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis) is a general cover term for the conjunction of two basic notions. The first notion is that languages are relative, that is, that they vary in their expression of concepts in noteworthy ways. The second notion is that the linguistic expression of concepts has some degree of ...

  12. PDF LINGUISTIC RELATIVITY

    The linguistic relativity hypothesis, the proposal that the particular language. we speak influences the way we think about reality, forms one part of the. broader question of how language influences thought. Despite long-standing. historical interest in the hypothesis, there is relatively litle empirical research.

  13. Clarification on Terminology of Linguistic Theories : r/Mcat

    Linguistic relativity/relativism is an interaction between language and the world where language influences perceptions, but the world can also influence language --> weak. (Hope this makes some sense) Determinism is more radical in its view as it states that solely language influence perception. It's less interactive than relativism, hence strong.

  14. Linguistic relativity

    Linguistic relativity asserts that language influences worldview or cognition.One form of linguistic relativity, linguistic determinism, regards peoples' languages as determining and influencing the scope of cultural perceptions of their surrounding world. [1]Several various colloquialisms refer to linguistic relativism: the Whorf hypothesis; the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis (/ s ə ˌ p ɪər ˈ ...

  15. PDF LNGS7006 Linguistic relativity Researchgate

    This idea of 'Linguistic. relativity' (LR), alternatively known as the 'Sapir-Whorf hypothesis', has become one of the most contentious and debated topics in Linguistics, vexatiously multi ...

  16. Linguistic Relativity in Conceptual Metaphors

    The linguistic relativity hypothesis claims that language does not strictly determine thought, but can shape speakers' cognition. Several studies have been conducted to test the linguistic relativity hypothesis in various domains, such as color, number, space, time, and musical pitch. Although the physiological basis of these concepts may be ...

  17. r/Mcat on Reddit: Are linguistic relativity and weak linguistic

    Help! "Weak linguistic determinism" is kind of redundant to say, because "weak" and "determinism" are clashing. It's linguistic relativity. The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, aka Whorfianism, is the general idea that language influences cognition. This idea is split into 2 parts. The weak version, which states that language merely ...

  18. Khan Academy

    Khanmigo is now free for all US educators! Plan lessons, develop exit tickets, and so much more with our AI teaching assistant.

  19. Linguistic relativities: Language diversity and modern thought ...

    this model, linguistic relativity became a 'hypothesis' that called for an experimental scientific con firmation never envisioned by the Boasians. Rather, Boas, Sapir, and Whorf engaged the inter meshing of language, culture, and habitual perception in particular societies to demonstrate the often extralinguistic workings of linguistic relativity.

  20. Toward a script relativity hypothesis: focused research ...

    The purpose of this paper is to extend the linguistic relativity hypothesis (i.e., the language we speak affects the way we think) to a script relativity hypothesis (i.e., the script in which we read influences our thought). Based on the rich body of knowledge in the science of reading that shows the effects of literacy on our cognitive processes, the foundation, rationale, and converging ...

  21. r/Mcat on Reddit: What is the difference between linguistic determinism

    The MCAT (Medical College Admission Test) is offered by the AAMC and is a required exam for admission to medical schools in the USA and Canada. /r/MCAT is a place for MCAT practice, questions, discussion, advice, social networking, news, study tips and more. ... Linguistic relativity hypothesis asserts and cognition and perceptions are ...

  22. Khan Academy

    Certain cookies and other technologies are essential in order to enable our Service to provide the features you have requested, such as making it possible for you to access our product and information related to your account.

  23. Can someone explain linguistic relativism? : r/Mcat

    Let me try to explain: linguistic relativism (also called linguistic determinism) doesn't actually tell you anything on its own. It is the different amounts language influences cognition and then there is the "strong" and "weak" hypotheses of this. So the strong hypothesis is the Whorfian-Sapir model, which is that language determines thought ...