- Case report
- Open access
- Published: 11 September 2017
A case of a four-year-old child adopted at eight months with unusual mood patterns and significant polypharmacy
- Magdalena Romanowicz ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-4916-0625 1 ,
- Alastair J. McKean 1 &
- Jennifer Vande Voort 1
BMC Psychiatry volume 17 , Article number: 330 ( 2017 ) Cite this article
46k Accesses
2 Citations
1 Altmetric
Metrics details
Long-term effects of neglect in early life are still widely unknown. Diversity of outcomes can be explained by differences in genetic risk, epigenetics, prenatal factors, exposure to stress and/or substances, and parent-child interactions. Very common sub-threshold presentations of children with history of early trauma are challenging not only to diagnose but also in treatment.
Case presentation
A Caucasian 4-year-old, adopted at 8 months, male patient with early history of neglect presented to pediatrician with symptoms of behavioral dyscontrol, emotional dysregulation, anxiety, hyperactivity and inattention, obsessions with food, and attachment issues. He was subsequently seen by two different child psychiatrists. Pharmacotherapy treatment attempted included guanfacine, fluoxetine and amphetamine salts as well as quetiapine, aripiprazole and thioridazine without much improvement. Risperidone initiated by primary care seemed to help with his symptoms of dyscontrol initially but later the dose had to be escalated to 6 mg total for the same result. After an episode of significant aggression, the patient was admitted to inpatient child psychiatric unit for stabilization and taper of the medicine.
Conclusions
The case illustrates difficulties in management of children with early history of neglect. A particular danger in this patient population is polypharmacy, which is often used to manage transdiagnostic symptoms that significantly impacts functioning with long term consequences.
Peer Review reports
There is a paucity of studies that address long-term effects of deprivation, trauma and neglect in early life, with what little data is available coming from institutionalized children [ 1 ]. Rutter [ 2 ], who studied formerly-institutionalized Romanian children adopted into UK families, found that this group exhibited prominent attachment disturbances, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), quasi-autistic features and cognitive delays. Interestingly, no other increases in psychopathology were noted [ 2 ].
Even more challenging to properly diagnose and treat are so called sub-threshold presentations of children with histories of early trauma [ 3 ]. Pincus, McQueen, & Elinson [ 4 ] described a group of children who presented with a combination of co-morbid symptoms of various diagnoses such as conduct disorder, ADHD, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression and anxiety. As per Shankman et al. [ 5 ], these patients may escalate to fulfill the criteria for these disorders. The lack of proper diagnosis imposes significant challenges in terms of management [ 3 ].
J is a 4-year-old adopted Caucasian male who at the age of 2 years and 4 months was brought by his adoptive mother to primary care with symptoms of behavioral dyscontrol, emotional dysregulation, anxiety, hyperactivity and inattention, obsessions with food, and attachment issues. J was given diagnoses of reactive attachment disorder (RAD) and ADHD. No medications were recommended at that time and a referral was made for behavioral therapy.
She subsequently took him to two different child psychiatrists who diagnosed disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD), PTSD, anxiety and a mood disorder. To help with mood and inattention symptoms, guanfacine, fluoxetine, methylphenidate and amphetamine salts were all prescribed without significant improvement. Later quetiapine, aripiprazole and thioridazine were tried consecutively without behavioral improvement (please see Table 1 for details).
No significant drug/substance interactions were noted (Table 1 ). There were no concerns regarding adherence and serum drug concentrations were not ordered. On review of patient’s history of medication trials guanfacine and methylphenidate seemed to have no effect on J’s hyperactive and impulsive behavior as well as his lack of focus. Amphetamine salts that were initiated during hospitalization were stopped by the patient’s mother due to significant increase in aggressive behaviors and irritability. Aripiprazole was tried for a brief period of time and seemed to have no effect. Quetiapine was initially helpful at 150 mg (50 mg three times a day), unfortunately its effects wore off quickly and increase in dose to 300 mg (100 mg three times a day) did not seem to make a difference. Fluoxetine that was tried for anxiety did not seem to improve the behaviors and was stopped after less than a month on mother’s request.
J’s condition continued to deteriorate and his primary care provider started risperidone. While initially helpful, escalating doses were required until he was on 6 mg daily. In spite of this treatment, J attempted to stab a girl at preschool with scissors necessitating emergent evaluation, whereupon he was admitted to inpatient care for safety and observation. Risperidone was discontinued and J was referred to outpatient psychiatry for continuing medical monitoring and therapy.
Little is known about J’s early history. There is suspicion that his mother was neglectful with feeding and frequently left him crying, unattended or with strangers. He was taken away from his mother’s care at 7 months due to neglect and placed with his aunt. After 1 month, his aunt declined to collect him from daycare, deciding she was unable to manage him. The owner of the daycare called Child Services and offered to care for J, eventually becoming his present adoptive parent.
J was a very needy baby who would wake screaming and was hard to console. More recently he wakes in the mornings anxious and agitated. He is often indiscriminate and inappropriate interpersonally, unable to play with other children. When in significant distress he regresses, and behaves as a cat, meowing and scratching the floor. Though J bonded with his adoptive mother well and was able to express affection towards her, his affection is frequently indiscriminate and he rarely shows any signs of separation anxiety.
At the age of 2 years and 8 months there was a suspicion for speech delay and J was evaluated by a speech pathologist who concluded that J was exhibiting speech and language skills that were solidly in the average range for age, with developmental speech errors that should be monitored over time. They did not think that issues with communication contributed significantly to his behavioral difficulties. Assessment of intellectual functioning was performed at the age of 2 years and 5 months by a special education teacher. Based on Bailey Infant and Toddler Development Scale, fine and gross motor, cognitive and social communication were all within normal range.
J’s adoptive mother and in-home therapist expressed significant concerns in regards to his appetite. She reports that J’s biological father would come and visit him infrequently, but always with food and sweets. J often eats to the point of throwing up and there have been occasions where he has eaten his own vomit and dog feces. Mother noticed there is an association between his mood and eating behaviors. J’s episodes of insatiable and indiscriminate hunger frequently co-occur with increased energy, diminished need for sleep, and increased speech. This typically lasts a few days to a week and is followed by a period of reduced appetite, low energy, hypersomnia, tearfulness, sadness, rocking behavior and slurred speech. Those episodes last for one to 3 days. Additionally, there are times when his symptomatology seems to be more manageable with fewer outbursts and less difficulty regarding food behaviors.
J’s family history is poorly understood, with his biological mother having a personality disorder and ADHD, and a biological father with substance abuse. Both maternally and paternally there is concern for bipolar disorder.
J has a clear history of disrupted attachment. He is somewhat indiscriminate in his relationship to strangers and struggles with impulsivity, aggression, sleep and feeding issues. In addition to early life neglect and possible trauma, J has a strong family history of psychiatric illness. His mood, anxiety and sleep issues might suggest underlying PTSD. His prominent hyperactivity could be due to trauma or related to ADHD. With his history of neglect, indiscrimination towards strangers, mood liability, attention difficulties, and heightened emotional state, the possibility of Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder (DSED) is likely. J’s prominent mood lability, irritability and family history of bipolar disorder, are concerning for what future mood diagnosis this portends.
As evidenced above, J presents as a diagnostic conundrum suffering from a combination of transdiagnostic symptoms that broadly impact his functioning. Unfortunately, although various diagnoses such as ADHD, PTSD, Depression, DMDD or DSED may be entertained, the patient does not fall neatly into any of the categories.
This is a case report that describes a diagnostic conundrum in a young boy with prominent early life deprivation who presented with multidimensional symptoms managed with polypharmacy.
A sub-threshold presentation in this patient partially explains difficulties with diagnosis. There is no doubt that negative effects of early childhood deprivation had significant impact on developmental outcomes in this patient, but the mechanisms that could explain the associations are still widely unknown. Significant family history of mental illness also predisposes him to early challenges. The clinical picture is further complicated by the potential dynamic factors that could explain some of the patient’s behaviors. Careful examination of J’s early life history would suggest such a pattern of being able to engage with his biological caregivers, being given food, being tended to; followed by periods of neglect where he would withdraw, regress and engage in rocking as a self-soothing behavior. His adoptive mother observed that visitations with his biological father were accompanied by being given a lot of food. It is also possible that when he was under the care of his biological mother, he was either attended to with access to food or neglected, left hungry and screaming for hours.
The current healthcare model, being centered on obtaining accurate diagnosis, poses difficulties for treatment in these patients. Given the complicated transdiagnostic symptomatology, clear guidelines surrounding treatment are unavailable. To date, there have been no psychopharmacological intervention trials for attachment issues. In patients with disordered attachment, pharmacologic treatment is typically focused on co-morbid disorders, even with sub-threshold presentations, with the goal of symptom reduction [ 6 ]. A study by dosReis [ 7 ] found that psychotropic usage in community foster care patients ranged from 14% to 30%, going to 67% in therapeutic foster care and as high as 77% in group homes. Another study by Breland-Noble [ 8 ] showed that many children receive more than one psychotropic medication, with 22% using two medications from the same class.
It is important to note that our patient received four different neuroleptic medications (quetiapine, aripiprazole, risperidone and thioridazine) for disruptive behaviors and impulsivity at a very young age. Olfson et al. [ 9 ] noted that between 1999 and 2007 there has been a significant increase in the use of neuroleptics for very young children who present with difficult behaviors. A preliminary study by Ercan et al. [ 10 ] showed promising results with the use of risperidone in preschool children with behavioral dyscontrol. Review by Memarzia et al. [ 11 ] suggested that risperidone decreased behavioral problems and improved cognitive-motor functions in preschoolers. The study also raised concerns in regards to side effects from neuroleptic medications in such a vulnerable patient population. Younger children seemed to be much more susceptible to side effects in comparison to older children and adults with weight gain being the most common. Weight gain associated with risperidone was most pronounced in pre-adolescents (Safer) [ 12 ]. Quetiapine and aripiprazole were also associated with higher rates of weight gain (Correll et al.) [ 13 ].
Pharmacokinetics of medications is difficult to assess in very young children with ongoing development of the liver and the kidneys. It has been observed that psychotropic medications in children have shorter half-lives (Kearns et al.) [ 14 ], which would require use of higher doses for body weight in comparison to adults for same plasma level. Unfortunately, that in turn significantly increases the likelihood and severity of potential side effects.
There is also a question on effects of early exposure to antipsychotics on neurodevelopment. In particular in the first 3 years of life there are many changes in developing brains, such as increase in synaptic density, pruning and increase in neuronal myelination to list just a few [ 11 ]. Unfortunately at this point in time there is a significant paucity of data that would allow drawing any conclusions.
Our case report presents a preschool patient with history of adoption, early life abuse and neglect who exhibited significant behavioral challenges and was treated with various psychotropic medications with limited results. It is important to emphasize that subthreshold presentation and poor diagnostic clarity leads to dangerous and excessive medication regimens that, as evidenced above is fairly common in this patient population.
Neglect and/or abuse experienced early in life is a risk factor for mental health problems even after adoption. Differences in genetic risk, epigenetics, prenatal factors (e.g., malnutrition or poor nutrition), exposure to stress and/or substances, and parent-child interactions may explain the diversity of outcomes among these individuals, both in terms of mood and behavioral patterns [ 15 , 16 , 17 ]. Considering that these children often present with significant functional impairment and a wide variety of symptoms, further studies are needed regarding diagnosis and treatment.
Abbreviations
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
Disruptive Mood Dysregulation Disorder
Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
Reactive Attachment disorder
Norman RE, Byambaa M, De R, Butchart A, Scott J, Vos T. The long-term health consequences of child physical abuse, emotional abuse, and neglect: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 2012;9(11):e1001349. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001349 . Epub 2012 Nov 27
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Kreppner JM, O'Connor TG, Rutter M, English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team. Can inattention/overactivity be an institutional deprivation syndrome? J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2001;29(6):513–28. PMID: 11761285
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Dejong M. Some reflections on the use of psychiatric diagnosis in the looked after or “in care” child population. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;15(4):589–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104510377705 .
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Pincus HA, McQueen LE, Elinson L. Subthreshold mental disorders: Nosological and research recommendations. In: Phillips KA, First MB, Pincus HA, editors. Advancing DSM: dilemmas in psychiatric diagnosis. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2003. p. 129–44.
Google Scholar
Shankman SA, Lewinsohn PM, Klein DN, Small JW, Seeley JR, Altman SE. Subthreshold conditions as precursors for full syndrome disorders: a 15-year longitudinal study of multiple diagnostic classes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:1485–94.
AACAP. Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with reactive attachment disorder of infancy and early childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:1206–18.
Article Google Scholar
dosReis S, Zito JM, Safer DJ, Soeken KL. Mental health services for youths in foster care and disabled youths. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(7):1094–9.
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Breland-Noble AM, Elbogen EB, Farmer EMZ, Wagner HR, Burns BJ. Use of psychotropic medications by youths in therapeutic foster care and group homes. Psychiatr Serv. 2004;55(6):706–8.
Olfson M, Crystal S, Huang C. Trends in antipsychotic drug use by very young, privately insured children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:13–23.
PubMed Google Scholar
Ercan ES, Basay BK, Basay O. Risperidone in the treatment of conduct disorder in preschool children without intellectual disability. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2011;5:10.
Memarzia J, Tracy D, Giaroli G. The use of antipsychotics in preschoolers: a veto or a sensible last option? J Psychopharmacol. 2014;28(4):303–19.
Safer DJ. A comparison of risperidone-induced weight gain across the age span. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2004;24:429–36.
Correll CU, Manu P, Olshanskiy V. Cardiometabolic risk of second-generation antipsychotic medications during first-time use in children and adolescents. JAMA. 2009;302:1765–73.
Kearns GL, Abdel-Rahman SM, Alander SW. Developmental pharmacology – drug disposition, action, and therapy in infants and children. N Engl J Med. 2003;349:1157–67.
Monk C, Spicer J, Champagne FA. Linking prenatal maternal adversity to developmental outcomes in infants: the role of epigenetic pathways. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(4):1361–76. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000764 . Review. PMID: 23062303
Cecil CA, Viding E, Fearon P, Glaser D, McCrory EJ. Disentangling the mental health impact of childhood abuse and neglect. Child Abuse Negl. 2016;63:106–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2016.11.024 . [Epub ahead of print] PMID: 27914236
Nemeroff CB. Paradise lost: the neurobiological and clinical consequences of child abuse and neglect. Neuron. 2016;89(5):892–909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.019 . Review. PMID: 26938439
Download references
Acknowledgements
We are also grateful to patient’s legal guardian for their support in writing this manuscript.
Availability of data and materials
Not applicable.
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
Mayo Clinic, Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, 200 1st SW, Rochester, MN, 55901, USA
Magdalena Romanowicz, Alastair J. McKean & Jennifer Vande Voort
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Contributions
MR, AJM, JVV conceptualized and followed up the patient. MR, AJM, JVV did literature survey and wrote the report and took part in the scientific discussion and in finalizing the manuscript. All the authors read and approved the final document.
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Magdalena Romanowicz .
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate, consent for publication.
Written consent was obtained from the patient’s legal guardian for publication of the patient’s details.
Competing interests
The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Cite this article.
Romanowicz, M., McKean, A.J. & Vande Voort, J. A case of a four-year-old child adopted at eight months with unusual mood patterns and significant polypharmacy. BMC Psychiatry 17 , 330 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1492-y
Download citation
Received : 20 December 2016
Accepted : 01 September 2017
Published : 11 September 2017
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1492-y
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
- Polypharmacy
- Disinhibited social engagement disorder
BMC Psychiatry
ISSN: 1471-244X
- General enquiries: [email protected]
"A Child's Anger Can Be a Warning"
How should parents respond when a child's anger escalates.
Posted January 26, 2012 | Reviewed by Devon Frye
- How Can I Manage My Anger?
- Take our Anger Management Test
- Find a therapist to heal from anger
- Anger in itself is not a bad thing, but children need to be educated about how to deal with and express their anger.
- Some cases of uncontrollable anger in children can be improved by professional intervention and medication.
- Risks of untreated, uncontrolled anger in children can include an escalation to violence from which they suffer permanent consequences.
Often, even well-meaning parents don't understand their child's angry behavior. Temper tantrums are normal for very young children, and lashing out with angry words is typical for adolescents. But what about threats or actions that go beyond predictable behavior?
What I too often see is that parents attempt to make excuses for their child, rather than dealing with or seeking help for their child's violent outbursts. As a child's anger escalates, some parents may try to placate him by giving in to his demands, or try to please him in an effort to calm him down. Some may rationalize that anger is a good thing and a child must be allowed to vent his anger and even behave wildly, because such behavior is an expression of how the child is feeling.
Of course, there is some truth to that way of thinking. Anger is not a bad thing in itself, and we all have the right to be angry when we are treated unfairly. But children need to be educated about how to deal with and express their anger when someone makes them mad or when something upsetting happens. Simply allowing a child's instinctual responses to take over is not always a good idea, especially when their anger seems out of control. And merely telling children to "get over it" or "be nice" doesn't teach them how to handle critical moments in their life. Teaching by example is always the most effective tool—it shows your child that you can be upset about something without losing control and that you can talk about why you're angry and then let it go. Parents can also teach children to:
- Talk to a trusted friend or relative about why they're angry
- Take some time to cool down when faced with an upsetting incident
- Slowly count to 10 when they feel themselves getting very angry
- Breathe deeply when they sense an angry outburst coming on
When a child's anger becomes uncontrollable
Some children have never been taught that there are limits to how they should express their anger. Sometimes, parents have not provided a healthy model for handling anger or have not wanted to constrain their child in any way. Other children are clearly not in control of their behavior, even though their parents may have been good role models and set forth reasonable limits. In either case, there may be medical or neurological reasons for a child's inability to control his anger. When parents' efforts are ineffective in dealing with a child's anger, a professional consultation is warranted. Medication might be necessary in some cases and can be prescribed by a child psychiatrist.
Causes and risks of uncontrollable anger in children
There is no diagnosis for angry behavior per se, but some of the conditions that involve uncontrollable anger include bipolar disorder , oppositional defiant disorder, and antisocial personality disorder .
Regardless of the label we place on a child's behavior, it is crucial for parents to realize that when their son's or daughter's anger is out of control, he or she needs to be treated by a professional. When the warning signs of uncontrollable anger go unheeded, a child's emotional flare-ups can spiral into violence that may result in tragedy—as in the recent case of a 10-year-old boy charged with murder for fatally stabbing his 12-year-old best friend.
Believing that a child's anger is "just a phase" that he or she will eventually outgrow is to deny what could be a serious problem. Before uncontrollable angry behavior escalates to a point of no return, parents can confront it and get the professional help they need.
Ana Nogales, Ph.D., is a clinical psychologist and well-known media personality, columnist, speaker, and advocate for victims of domestic violence.
- Find a Therapist
- Find a Treatment Center
- Find a Psychiatrist
- Find a Support Group
- Find Online Therapy
- United States
- Brooklyn, NY
- Chicago, IL
- Houston, TX
- Los Angeles, CA
- New York, NY
- Portland, OR
- San Diego, CA
- San Francisco, CA
- Seattle, WA
- Washington, DC
- Asperger's
- Bipolar Disorder
- Chronic Pain
- Eating Disorders
- Passive Aggression
- Personality
- Goal Setting
- Positive Psychology
- Stopping Smoking
- Low Sexual Desire
- Relationships
- Child Development
- Self Tests NEW
- Therapy Center
- Diagnosis Dictionary
- Types of Therapy
When we fall prey to perfectionism, we think we’re honorably aspiring to be our very best, but often we’re really just setting ourselves up for failure, as perfection is impossible and its pursuit inevitably backfires.
- Emotional Intelligence
- Gaslighting
- Affective Forecasting
- Neuroscience
An official website of the United States government
Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.
Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.
- Publications
- Account settings
- Advanced Search
- Journal List
Parenting Stress and Child Behavior Problems: A Transactional Relationship Across Time
Cameron l neece, shulamite a green, bruce l baker.
- Author information
- Copyright and License information
Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Cameron Leigh Neece, who is now at Loma Linda University, Department of Psychology, 11130 Anderson St., Suite 102, Loma Linda, CA 92354. [email protected]
Parenting stress and child behavior problems have been posited to have a transactional effect on each other across development. However, few studies have tested this model empirically. The authors investigated the relationship between parenting stress and child behavior problems from ages 3 to 9 years old among 237 children, 144 of whom were typically developing and 93 who were identified as developmentally delayed. Behavior problems and parenting stress covaried significantly across time for both groups of children. Cross-lagged panel analyses generally supported a bidirectional relationship between parenting stress and child behavior problems for mothers and fathers.
Keywords: developmental delay, intellectual disability, parenting stress, behavior problems
The transactional model of development asserts that development is not the sum of individual mechanisms but the product of ongoing interactions between the individual and the environment, with a particular focus on bidirectional or reciprocal effects ( Belsky, 1984 ; Gottleib, 2007 ; Sameroff 2009 ). This conceptual perspective has been used frequently to understand the influence of parent–child interactions on child development. Reciprocal interactions between child and parent factors have been associated, for example, with the development of temperament ( Pesonen et al., 2008 ), internalizing problems ( Fanti, Henrich, Brookmeyer, & Kupermine, 2008 ), externalizing problems ( Gross, Shaw, & Moilanen, 2008 ; Zhang, Chen, Zhang, Zhou, & Wu, 2008 ), emotional adjustment ( VanderValk, de Goede, Spruijt, & Meeus, 2007 ), self-regulation ( Brody & Ge, 2001 ), and substance use ( Wills & Dishion, 2004 ). Parent and family factors that have been implicated in these developmental processes include parental depression ( Gross et al., 2008 ), marital distress ( VanderValk et al., 2007 ), parenting practices ( Brody & Ge, 2001 ), and parent–child relationships ( Fanti et al., 2008 ; Zhang et al., 2008 ). Most of these studies have focused on middle childhood and adolescence and used limited longitudinal data (i.e., two or three time points).
We sought to extend research examining this transactional model by (a) examining another critical parent factor, parenting stress, and (b) using substantial longitudinal data (seven time points) that included significant developmental transition points (e.g., preschool to late elementary school), generating a more complete understanding of this developmental process. We analyzed the transactional relationship between parental stress and child behavior problems across early and middle childhood (ages 3 to 9 years). Although it is likely that parenting stress and child behavior problems have a mutually escalating reciprocal interaction over time, few studies have examined this relationship. Our sample included not only children with typical development but also children with developmental delays, because the latter are more likely to have elevated levels of behavior problems as well as parents with high levels of stress, allowing us to examine the full spectrum of these constructs.
Children With Developmental Delays as a Risk Group
There is consistent evidence that children with cognitive delays are more likely to have significant behavior problems and to develop psychopathology. Studies have found heightened externalizing and internalizing behavior problems relative to typically developing children ( Baker, Blacher, Crnic, & Edelbrock, 2002 ; Emerson & Einfeld, 2010 ; Merrell & Holland, 1997 ). In early analyses of the present longitudinal sample at child age 3 years, we found that 26.1% of the children with developmental delays exhibited clinical levels of behavior problems compared with 8.3% of children with typical development ( Baker et al., 2002 ). Early behavior problems are a particularly important risk factor because they have been associated with the development of psychopathology among children with and without early developmental risk. In additional analyses with the present sample, 54% of typically developing children and 67% of children with developmental delays who had clinical levels of externalizing behavior problems at age 3 met diagnostic criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder at age 5 ( Baker, Neece, Fenning, Crnic, & Blacher, 2010 ).
The development of psychopathology is a significant concern for individuals with developmental delays. Epidemiological studies have found that from one third to one half of children and adolescents with delays meet criteria for a comorbid psychiatric disorder ( Cormack, Brown, & Hastings, 2000 ; Dekker & Koot, 2003 ; Koskentausta, Iivanainen, & Almqvist, 2007 ). Evidence from several studies that have included a comparison group with typical cognitive development has suggested that about 2.5 to more than 4 times as many children with cognitive delays have serious behavior–emotional problems as those with typical development ( Baker et al., 2010 ; Dekker, Koot, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 2002 ; deRuiter et al., 2008 ; Emerson & Hatton, 2007 ).
Thus, it is clear that high levels of behavior problems and psychopathology are significant risk factors for children with delays. It is likely that these child risk factors interact with the environment over time, which either intensifies the risk or serves as a protective factor. In the present study, we focused on one environmental variable, parenting stress, which is hypothesized to exacerbate the development of behavior problems over time.
Parenting Stress as an Environmental Risk Factor
High parenting stress is an important environmental risk variable. It has been associated with numerous undesirable outcomes, including parent depression (Anastopoulos, Guevremont, Shelton, & DuPaul, 1992; Deater-Deckard et al., 1998 ; Hastings, Daley, Burns, & Beck, 2006 ), marital conflict ( Kersh, Hedvat, Hauser-Cram, Warfield, 2006 ; Suárez & Baker, 1997 ), poorer physical health ( Eisenhower, Baker, & Blacher, 2009 ; Oelofsen & Richardson, 2006 ), less effective parenting ( Coldwell, Pike, & Dunn, 2006 ; Crnic, Gaze, & Hoffman, 2005 ), and, of most importance to the present study, increased child behavior problems ( Baker et al., 2003 ; Briggs-Gowan, Carter, Skuban, & Horwitz, 2001 ; Donenberg & Baker, 1993 ; Johnson & Mash, 2001 ).
Children with delays are more likely to have family environments with high levels of parenting stress. Parents of children with delays typically report more parenting stress than parents of typically developing children ( Baker et al., 2003 ; Emerson, 2003 ; Hauser-Cram, Warfield, Shonkoff, & Kraus, 2001 ). Although there is some evidence that the stress experienced by parents of children with developmental delays can be chronic, there is marked individual variation in its trajectory over the life course ( Glidden & Schoolcraft, 2003 ).
Transactional Model: Parenting Stress and Child Behavior Problems
Although there is some support for a transactional relationship between parenting stress and child behavior problems, very few studies have examined this relationship, even in families of children with typical cognitive development. Some studies of children with developmental delays have found that behavior problems mediated the relationship between child developmental status and parenting stress ( Baker et al., 2002 ; Hauser-Cram et al., 2001 ; Herring et al., 2006 ). In these studies, when child behavior problems were accounted for, there was no longer a significant relationship between child cognitive delay and parenting stress.
In addition, limited longitudinal analyses have suggested that the relationship between behavior problems and parenting stress is bidirectional—that many elevations in behavior problems lead to increases in parenting stress over time, and high parenting stress leads to increases in behavior problems in children ( Baker et al., 2003 ) and adults ( Orsmond, Seltzer, Krauss, & Hong, 2003 ).
However, there is little known about the trajectories of behavior problems and parenting stress across time, and, to our knowledge, no study has examined the associations among these trajectories across multiple time points. Furthermore, previous studies have tested each direction of effect (early behavior problems to later parenting stress and early parenting stress to later behavior problems) independently rather than examining models where both directions of effect are tested simultaneously.
The general theory of psychological stress may be helpful in understanding the reciprocal relationship between parenting stress and behavior problems. The stress process includes four components: (a) an external, causal event or agent; (b) a cognitive appraisal of the event or agent to determine whether it is unpleasant; (c) coping mechanisms to reduce the unpleasant effect of the event or agent; and (d) consequential effects, or stress reactions ( Lazarus, 1993 ). Child behavior problems are thought to be a causal agent of stress and, thus, are hypothesized to have a direct link to parents’ level of stress. In contrast, the effect from parental stress to child behavior problems may be less direct. Parenting behavior is thought to be a stress reaction that mediates the relationship between stress and child behavior problems ( Deater-Deckard, 1998 ). Parenting stress has been linked to less responsive, more authoritarian, and more neglectful parenting ( Belsky, Woodward, & Crnic, 1996 ; Conger, Patterson, & Ge, 1996; Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996 ; McBride & Mills, 1994), which, in turn, has been associated with poorer developmental outcomes for the child ( Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994 ). However, despite multiple studies supporting the associations among parental stress, parenting behavior, and child outcomes, little research has explicitly tested this full meditational model ( Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996 ). This is an important direction for future research.
Differential Effects for Mothers and Fathers
The majority of studies examining child behavior problems and parenting stress have included only mothers. We have found high agreement among mother and father reports of behavior problems at child ages 3 and 4 years, particularly among the parents of children with delays, as well as similar relationships between child behavior problems and parenting stress for mothers and fathers ( Baker et al., 2002 , 2003 ). However, fathers can form different relationships with their children ( Phares, 1996 ), have different opportunities to observe them ( Hay et al., 1999 ), and have different experiences and associated outcomes of parenting stress ( Roggman et al., 2004 ). Thus, we do not know whether the relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress over time is the same for mothers and fathers. In the present study, we addressed the degree of similarity among mothers and fathers in assessment of child behavior problems in early and middle childhood, their experience of parental stress during this time, and the relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress across development.
Current Study
In the current study, we analyzed the transactional relationship between parenting stress and child behavior problems across early and middle childhood (ages 3–9 years). The parenting stress variable was derived from a measure of how a particular child affects the family (Family Impact Questionnaire [FIQ]; Donenberg & Baker 1993 ); the score reflects parents’ reports of negative feelings about parenting and negative impact on their relationships with others. The behavior problems variable was the total score on the parent-completed Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 2000 ; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001 ). We examined (a) the trajectories of child behavior problems and parenting stress across seven time points, (b) whether child behavior problems and parenting stress were related across time, (c) the direction of effect between child behavior problems and parenting stress over time, and (d) whether these relationships differed for mothers and fathers.
Participants included 237 families recruited into the Collaborative Family Study, a longitudinal study of young children, with samples drawn from southern California ( n = 196) and central Pennsylvania ( n = 41). The Collaborative Family Study was based at three universities: Pennsylvania State University, the University of California, Los Angeles, and the University of California, Riverside. Most families ( n = 218) completed an intake assessment near the child’s third birthday ( M = 35.2 months; SD = 3.0). An additional 19 families of children with developmental delays entered the study at child age 5 years; they did not differ from the original developmental delays sample on any study variable. We use the term developmental delay rather than the more formal diagnosis of intellectual disability for this sample because (a) the cognitive assessment was conducted on the children when they were young, likely resulting in a less stable classification over time than with older children and (b) the groupings were based on IQ alone.
Families of children with developmental delays were recruited primarily through agencies that provide and purchase diagnostic and intervention services for persons with developmental disabilities. In California, almost all families with young children with developmental delays register for services with one of a network of regional centers. Children in the developmental delays group at intake were all in the borderline to moderate ranges of cognitive delay on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development ( Bayley, 1993 ); they were ambulatory and not diagnosed with autism. Families of children with typical development were recruited primarily through local preschools and daycare programs. Additional typical development selection criteria were that the child score in the range of normal cognitive development and not have been born prematurely or have any developmental disability. In recruiting participants, school and agency personnel mailed brochures describing the study to families who met selection criteria and interested parents contacted the research center.
In the present study, we classified children based on the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales ( Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1986 ) at age 5 years. Children were classified as having developmental delays (IQ ≤ 70; n = 60), borderline (IQ = 71–84, n = 33), or typically developing (IQ ≥ 85; n = 144). Children categorized as having developmental delays or borderline were combined in the present analyses and referred to as developmentally delayed . The present sample was drawn from an initial total sample of 279 families. To be included in these analyses, families must have completed the two key measures (behavior problems and parenting stress) together at a minimum of two time points. The 237 families included in the study met these criteria. These families did not differ from the 42 families who did not meet these criteria on 16 variables examined: delay status, the initial behavior problem (CBCL total score), and stress (FIQ negative) scores, in addition to the demographic variables in Table 1 . The sample size at each age was as follows: n , age 3 = 217; n , age 4 = 212; n , age 5 = 232; n , age 6 = 195; n , age 7 = 187; n , age 8 = 167; and n , age 9 = 171. Overall, 72% of families continued in the sample from their first assessment to the 9-year assessment. The families who continued did not differ from dropout families on any of the 16 variables described above.
Demographics by Delay Status Group at Child Age 3 (n = 237)
p < .10.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics at child age 3, by group status (developmental delays, typical development). In the combined sample, there were more boys (57.8%) than girls. The majority of the mothers were Caucasian, non-Hispanic (60.1%), whereas 16.5% were Hispanic, 6.9% African American, 2.8% Asian American, and 13.8% self-classified as “other”. The socioeconomic status was generally high; 51.8% of families had an annual income above $50,000 (in 1998–2000 U.S. dollars), and the average years of schooling was 3 years of college for mothers and fathers. The two status groups did not differ significantly on child gender, child race–ethnicity, fathers’ age, parents’ race–ethnicity, parents’ health, marital status, or family income. However, mothers and fathers in the typically developing group completed significantly more years of school, mothers of typically developing children were slightly older, and families of typically developing children had a higher family income on average. In addition, children in the typically developing group were reported to have better physical health compared with children in the developmental delays group.
Procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the three universities involved. The primary data for this study were obtained though parent questionnaires at child ages 3–9 years. At child ages 3, 5, and 9 years, the family came into the center for an assessment, and at child ages 3, 4, 6, 7, and 8 years investigators conducted a home visit. During the center assessment at child age 5 years, measures were taken of child intelligence (Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Thorndike et al., 1986 ) and family demographics (based on interview with the mother). The questionnaire on parenting stress was mailed to the family and completed prior to the home or center visit, whereas the measure of behavior problems was completed during the home or center visit. Thus, administration of the two measures was from several days to several weeks apart.
Stanford-Binet IV (SB-IV)
Children’s cognitive ability was evaluated with the Stanford-Binet IV ( Thorndike et al., 1986 ), a widely used assessment instrument with sound psychometric properties. The SB-IV yields an IQ score with a normative mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. It is particularly well suited to the evaluation of children with delays because the examiner adapts starting points according to the child’s developmental level. Child cognitive status grouping (developmental delays vs. typical development) was based on SB-IV scores at child age 5.
Family Impact Questionnaire
The FIQ ( Donenberg & Baker, 1993 ) is a 50-item questionnaire that asks about the “child’s impact on the family compared to the impact other children his/her age have on their families” (e.g., Item 1: “My child is more stressful”). Parents endorse items on a 4-point scale ranging from not at all (1) to very much (4). Although there are six scales, of interest here are two scales that are combined into a 20-item negative-impact composite score (α = .87 each for mother and father reports at age 5). This FIQ negative-impact score is considered an indicator of parenting stress. It was designed to avoid the circular reasoning of stress measures that ask about child challenges and then infer parenting stress from these (e.g., Parenting Stress Index; Abidin, 1990 ). However, although conceptually different, FIQ negative-impact scores have been found to relate highly to the Parenting Stress Index Child Domain scores on the Parenting Stress Index ( r = .84) obtained from mothers of young, typically developing children ( Donenberg & Baker, 1993 ). This measure was administered annually from child ages 3–9 years.
CBCL for Ages 1½–5 years and Ages 6–18 years
Two versions of the CBCL were used (ages 1.5–5: Achenbach, 2000 ; ages 6–18: Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001 ). For the first three assessments (ages 3–5 years), the preschool version (for ages 1.5–5 years; 99 item) was used, and for the remaining four assessments (ages 6–9 years), the child version of the CBCL was used (for ages 6–18 years, 113 items). Each CBCL item indicates a child problem (e.g., fails to finish things he/she starts, temper tantrums or hot temper, sleeps more than most kids). The mother and father completed this questionnaire during the home or center visit, and for each item the respondent indicated whether it was not true (0), somewhat or sometimes true (1), or very true or often true (2), at that time point or within the previous 2 months. Items contained in the total problem score were used in the present analyses; alpha was 0.94 for mothers and fathers at age 3 years. The two CBCL versions have very high overlap in items, and previous studies have found them to be highly equivalent (e.g., Tan, 2011 ). However, because they differ in total number of items, we divided the sum score by the number of items and used this mean item score in analyses that included data from the CBCL.
Data Analytic Plan
Demographic variables listed in Table 1 that had a significant relationship ( p < .05) with one or more of the independent variables and one or more of the dependent variables were tested as covariates in the analyses. Covariates were retained in the final model if they predicted the dependent variable at p < .10.
To examine the trajectories of child behavior problems and parenting stress over time, as well as the relationship between the two variables, multilevel growth model analyses were conducted using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002 ). HLM analyses were used to examine (a) whether there was a significant change in child behavior problems and/or parenting stress over time, (b) whether the two variables changed in similar ways over time, and (c) whether there were status-group differences in the slope of each variable and the covariation of the two variables over time.
To examine the first question (i.e., significant change over time in each group), we first examined the best model of the rate of change. A linear slope term was first added to the model, and, then, quadratic and cubic terms were added in a stepwise hierarchical fashion to examine whether they significantly improved the fit of the model (i.e., the deviance parameter). In all cases, the best fit model was that which included only the intercept and linear slope term. Thus, we conducted growth models by including only an intercept (representing the dependent variable at Time 1), slope (representing the linear rate of change of the dependent variable across ages 3–9), and status (typical development vs. developmental delays). To examine the second question, conditional time-varying predictor growth models were run to test whether parenting stress and behavior problems covaried significantly over time (ages 3–9). The conditional time-varying predictor models differed from the initial growth models in that they included either behavior problems as a covariate of parenting stress over time or parenting stress as a covariate of behavior problems over time. A significant finding would indicate that the two variables (parenting stress and child behavior problems) covaried across time. The conditional models also included relevant demographic covariates. Specifically, family income was included as a covariate in the model examining father-reported stress as a time-varying covariate of child behavior problems; no other covariates were significant at p < .1 in any of the time-varying models.
In both the initial growth models and the conditional time-varying models, status was coded such that the typically developing group = 0 and the developmental delays group = 1, so that intercept coefficients pertained to the significance for the typically developing group, and the Intercept × Status interactions tested whether there was a significant difference between groups. When analyses showed a significant difference between groups (i.e., a significant interaction term), follow-up analyses were conducted with status recoded as developmental delays group = 0 and typically developing group = 1 to test for a significant relationship between the predictor and outcome variables in the developmental delays group.
Cross-lagged panel analyses were conducted to investigate the direction of the relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress across seven time points (annual assessments at ages 3–9). Child developmental status was included in these analyses as a covariate in predicting stress and behavior problems at Time 1 (age 3). Cross-lagged analyses allowed simultaneous examination of the two pathways of interest (early child behavior problems to later parenting stress and early parenting stress to later child behavior problems). There were six sets of cross-effects tested in these models (e.g., behavior problems at age 3 predicting stress at age 4 and stress at age 3 predicting behavior problems at age 4; behavior problems at age 4 predicting stress at age 5 and stress at age 4 predicting behavior problems at age 5). This approach differs from a regression analysis in that both dependent variables (behavior problems and parenting stress) are entered into the model and allowed to correlate. This is a more conservative analysis that accounts for the multicollinearity between the two dependent variables, leaving less variance in the dependent variables to be explained by the independent variables. Models were run separately for mother-report and father-report data across the seven time points. To address the problem of shared method variance, two additional models were conducted that mismatched informants of parenting stress and child behavior problems (mother report of stress and father report of children behavior problems, father report of stress and mother report of child behavior problems). Similar to the HLM analyses described above, to be included in the cross-lagged analyses families had to have at least two time points of data for both the CBCL and the FIQ. Cross-lagged models are often used in social science research and have been used in previous research with families of children with intellectual disabilities ( Greenberg, Seltzer, Hong, Orsmond, 2006 ; Neece & Baker, 2008 ; Neece, Blacher, & Baker, 2010 ).
The distributions of the primary child behavior problem and parenting stress variables were examined at each time point. Data points that were more than three standard deviations above or below the mean of a variable were considered to be outliers. As suggested by Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2002) , all outliers were set equal to plus or minus 3 standard deviations from the mean to reduce the influence of extreme data points on the results. Across all 28 behavior problem and stress variables (i.e., mother and father data for each of seven time points for both variables) and all participants, there were 26 outliers and all were adjusted downward. In addition, preliminary analyses examined the descriptive statistics for the behavior problems and stress variables, which are reported in Table 2 .
Descriptive Statistics for Stress and Behavior Problem Variables
Multilevel Growth Model Analyses
Child behavior problems.
First, growth models were used to examine the linear slope of child behavior problems across seven yearly time points for each status group. As in regression analyses, because Time 1 (age 3 years) was set to 0, the intercept (initial start point of the trajectory) of each model indicated the mean score at child age 3 for either child behavior problems or parenting stress. Results of the growth models are displayed in Table 3 .
Results of Unconditional Growth Models
Note. CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist ( Achenbach, 2000 ; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001 ); FIQ = Family Impact Questionnaire ( Donenberg & Baker 1993 ). Intercept and slope parameters are presented with standard error in parentheses.
Both mother and father models had significant negative slope parameters, indicating that behavior problems decreased significantly over time. In both models the developmental delays group had higher initial behavior problems than the typically developing group, but, only in the mother model, the developmental delays group had a significantly different slope (i.e., CBCL scores rated by mothers decreased more each year for the developmental delays group). There was significant individual variability in both the initial behavior problem scores and the trajectory of behavior problems, as shown by the significant variance parameters.
Conditional time-varying predictor growth models (see Table 4 ) were used to examine additional predictors of child behavior problems across time. For these analyses, family income was included as a covariate in the father-report model. For the mother-report model, initial analyses showed that time-varying parenting stress did not have a significant variance component (i.e., did not randomly vary across individuals) after accounting for variance due to change in time, so stress was entered as a fixed (not randomly varying) variable in this model. However, parenting stress did have a significant variance component for the father-report model, so the variance component was included in this model. Both mother- and father-reported stress significantly covaried over time with child behavior problems after controlling for the change over time in behavior problems.
Multilevel Growth Model Results for Mother- and Father-Reported Stress as Time-Varying Covariates of Child Behavior Problems (n=237)
In summary, results supported covariation between parenting stress and behavior problems across time, in addition to the decrease in behavior problems across time. Figure 1 depicts the CBCL and FIQ scores from ages 3 to 9 years old. To have both measures on a similar scale, the CBCL average item scores, which were quite small, were multiplied 100; therefore, they do not represent true raw scores. There was not a significant difference between the developmental delays and typically developing groups in the covariation over time of behavior problems and stress.
Marginal means of maternal parenting stress and child behavior problems for ages 3–9 years in the typically developing and developmental delays groups. Top panel: typically developing group; bottom panel: developmental delays group. Beh probs = behavioral problems.
Parenting stress
Unconditional growth models for parenting stress are displayed in Table 3 . In predicting parenting stress, both mother- and father-report models had significant negative slope parameters, indicating a significant decrease in parenting stress over time. The developmental delays group had significantly higher initial levels of parenting stress than the typically developing group did. There were no significant group differences in slope.
Conditional growth models (see Table 5 ) were then conducted to examine additional predictors of parenting stress across time. Initial analyses showed that time-varying behavior problems had a significant variance component for the mother but not the father model after accounting for variance due to change in time, so behavior problems was entered as a fixed (not randomly varying) variable for the father model. Child behavior problems significantly covaried over time with both mother- and father-reported stress after controlling for the change over time in parenting stress. In the father model only, this relationship was stronger for the developmental delays group at the trend level.
Multilevel Growth Model Results for Child Behavior Problems as Time-Varying Covariates of Mother- and Father-Reported Stress (n =237)
Note. CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist ( Achenbach, 2000 ; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001 ).
After the additional predictor of time-varying child behavior problems was entered into the models, the slope of parenting stress was no longer significant, and, in fact, the slope of the developmental delays group significantly differed from that of the typically developing group. Follow-up analyses indicated that the developmental delays group slope was significant and positive, suggesting that, for this group, parenting stress not accounted for by behavior problems was found to increase over time. In other words, parenting stress decreased over time for both groups in the unconditional model, but after behavior problems were entered into the model, the residual of the parenting stress slope increased for the developmental delays group. However, for the typically developing group, time did not predict changes in parenting stress in addition to child behavior problems. As in the previous model, results supported covariation between behavior problems and parenting stress across time, in addition to the effect of time on parenting stress.
Cross-lagged panel analyses
Cross-lagged panel analyses were used to examine the bidirectional effects of parenting stress and child behavior problems over time. Mplus was used to test two 7-wave cross-lagged models, one for mother reports and one for father reports. Child intellectual status was included as a covariate in predicting both the stress and behavior problems at Time 1. The dependent variables, parenting stress and children’s behavior problems, were measured at child ages 4–9 years. Predictor variables included parenting stress and child behavior problems from the preceding time point (e.g., when dependent variables were stress and behavior problems at age 4, predictor variables were stress and behavior problems at age 3). None of the tested covariates predicted either dependent variable at p < .10, and, thus, no covariates were included in the final cross-lagged models.
Figure 2 shows the cross-lagged panel analysis for the model using mother-report data. There was high stability for maternal parenting stress across all seven time points (ages 3–4: β = .78, p < .001; ages 4–5: β = .83, p < .001; ages 5–6: β = .62, p <.001; ages 6–7: β =.76, p <.001; ages 7–8: β = .78, p < .001; and ages 8–9: β = .74, p < .001). Child behavior problems were also highly stable across time, with the exception of ages 8–9, where the stability effect was nonsignificant (ages 3–4: β = .62, p < .001; ages 4–5: β = .57, p < .001; ages 5–6: β = .70, p < .001; ages 6–7: β = .63, p <.001; ages 7–8: β =.71, p <.001; and ages 8–9: β = .02, p = ns ).
Cross-lagged panel analysis model predicting child behavior problems and mother stress from child ages 3–9 years. FIQ = Family Impact Questionnaire ( Donenberg & Baker 1993 ); CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist ( Achenbach, 2000 ; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001 ).
Table 6 reports the cross-lagged effects from early child behavior to later parenting stress (in boldface). Three out of the six cross-lagged effects were significant. Regarding the effect from early parenting stress to later behavior problems, four of the six cross-lagged effects were significant. These results are reported in Table 7 (in boldface).
Cross-Effects From Child Behavior Problems to Later Parental Stress
Note. Boldfaced numerals represent cross-lagged effects with the same reporter.
Cross Effects From Parental Stress to Later Child Behavior Problems
Results using father-report data are shown in Figure 3 . All stability effects were significant for father’s parenting stress (ages 3–4: β = .71, p < .001; ages 4–5: β =.68, p <.001; ages 5–6: β =.79, p < .001; ages 6–7: β = .79, p < .001; ages 7–8: β = .67, p < .001; and ages 8–9: β = .80, p < .001) and child behavior problems (ages 3–4: β = .60, p <.001; ages 4–5: β =.58, p <.001; ages 5–6: β = .53, p < .001; ages 6–7: β =.60, p <.001; ages 7–8: β = .62, p < .001; and ages 8–9: β = .71, p < .001). As shown in Table 6 , four out of the six cross-lagged effects from early child behavior problems to later paternal parenting stress were significant. All six cross-lagged effects were significant from early parenting stress to later behavior problems (see Table 7 ).
Cross-lagged panel analysis model predicting child behavior problems and father stress from child ages 3–9 years. FIQ = Family Impact Questionnaire ( Donenberg & Baker 1993 ); CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist ( Achenbach, 2000 ; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001 ).
For both mother- and father-report models, Wald tests were conducted to test for differences in the cross-lagged effect parameters at each time point. This statistical technique tested whether the path from early behavior problems to later parenting stress was significantly different from the path from early parenting stress to later behavior problems at each cross-lagged time point. None of the cross-lagged effects were significantly different from each other in the mother- or father-report models.
To address the limitation of shared method variance, two additional cross-lagged panel analyses were conducted using mismatched reporters. These results are also reported in Tables 6 and 7 . In the model using mother reports of parenting stress and father reports of child behavior problems, four of the six cross-effects from early behavior problems to later parenting stress were significant, and four of the six cross-effects from early parenting stress to later parenting behavior problems were significant. In the model using father reports of parenting stress and mother reports of child behavior problems, two of the six cross-effects from early behavior problems to later parenting stress were significant and two were marginally significant, and three of the six cross-effects from early parenting stress to later behavior problems were significant and two were marginally significant. In summary, results provided some support for a bidirectional relationship between parenting stress and child behavior problems across time for both mothers and fathers.
In keeping with the transactional model of development, we focused on the reciprocal relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress across time. Overall, our findings provided converging evidence of a transactional relationship between these two variables across early and middle childhood. Results suggested that parenting stress is both an antecedent and consequence of child behavior problems. Simultaneously, child behavior problems are an antecedent and consequence of parenting stress. These variables appear to have a mutually escalating, or deescalating, effect on each other over time. We compared children without and with developmental delays to examine whether the relationship between behavior problems and parenting stress over time differed between families of children with typical development and those at developmental risk, and we found that the transactional relationship observed appears to be similar for children with and without developmental delays.
The study approach contributes to the field of developmental psychopathology by testing the transactional model using substantial longitudinal data, using a large sample of children at two levels of developmental risk, and examining processes within fathers as well as mothers. Our first set of analyses examined the trajectories of child behavior problems and parenting stress from ages 3–9 as well as the relationship between the trajectories (i.e., the covariance) using time-varying predictor analyses conducted with HLM. Behavior problems decreased across childhood for both groups (typically developing and developmental delays), consistent with other research showing a decrease in the level of problem behaviors across time (de Ruiter et al., 2007; McCarthy & Boyd, 2001 ; Wallander, Dekker, & Koot, 2003 ). This may be accounted for by individual developmental variables as well as environmental variables. As children develop, they acquire cognitive- and emotion-regulation skills that enable them to inhibit inappropriate behaviors, leading to a decrease in behavior problems ( Baker et al., 2002 , 2003 ; Eisenberg et al., 2001 ; Hill, Degnan, Calkins, & Keane, 2006 ; Olson, Sameroff, Lunkenheimer, & Kerr, 2009 ). In addition, the decline in behavior problems may be attributable to changes in the environment. Based on the graphical representation of the data, the largest decrease in behavior problems was from ages 5 to 6, when children typically entered full days of school, which is consistent with other studies showing a significant decrease in the level of behavior problems from preschool to elementary school entry ( Combs-Ronto, Olson, Lunkenheimer, & Sameroff, 2009 ; Kerr, Lunkenheimer, & Olson, 2007 ). The new behavioral demands of a more structured school environment may help to reduce child behavior problems. Furthermore, behavior problems decreased more rapidly for the developmental delays group than for the typically developing group; this was likely due to the group with developmental delays starting out higher but might be related to the higher number of services this group is likely to receive.
We also examined changes in parenting stress from ages 3–9 years, controlling for time-varying behavior problems, and found that stress appeared to decrease over time only for parents of typically developing children as a group. Our findings suggest that the effect of time on stress in the typically developing group was fully accounted for by changes in behavior problems. After behavior problems were accounted for, there was a positive slope in parenting stress across time for the developmental delays group. Although it is possible this was a statistical artifact due to the high correlations between time and behavior problems, it is also possible that variability in parenting stress slope not explained by behavior problems was increasing over time among parents of children with developmental delays. One possible explanation for these findings is that parents of children with delays may be more likely to have recurrent and new stressors that maintain and even increase stress levels across time. For example, school entry may be a particularly difficult time for parents of children with developmental delays. This is often when parents make peer comparisons and realize how far behind their child is, resulting in greater parenting stress. A previous study with the current sample found that the number of children in the developmental delays group who were mainstreamed decreased significantly from school entry at age 6 (40.5%) to second grade at age 8 (25.7%), suggesting that, on average, parents became increasingly aware of their child’s need for special services during this time ( Blacher, Baker, & Eisenhower, 2009 ). Furthermore, parents of children with delays are faced with many challenges across their child’s lifespan, including overcoming the disappointments related to the original diagnosis, securing school placements, and learning to navigate the health and educational systems ( Chen & Tang, 1997 ; Floyd et al., 1996 ; Glidden, 1989 ). Often, the sources of stress move beyond the child to include the service delivery system.
We also investigated whether behavior problems and parenting stress changed in similar ways across time. We found that parenting stress and child behavior problems covaried significantly across development. Child developmental status did not moderate the relationship between behavior problems and stress over time, which was congruent with past studies showing that cognitive functioning has an indirect effect on parenting stress that is accounted for by child behavior problems (e.g., Baker et al., 2003 ). These results provided support for a strong relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress over time; however, the direction of effect between these two was unclear.
We also investigated the direction of the relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress across early and middle childhood (ages 3–9), using cross-lagged panel analyses. Multiple studies examining the relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress among children with and without developmental delays have claimed that the effect between these two variables is bidirectional. However, to our knowledge, no study has tested these two directions of effect simultaneously, which is critical in determining whether this relationship is truly transactional. The present, more conservative analyses provided support for a bidirectional relationship. We observed significant cross-lagged effects, from initial parenting stress to later child behavior problems and from initial child behavior problems to later parenting stress. These findings were particularly interesting, given the high stability of child behavior problems and parenting stress over time, which affords little change to predict.
An earlier and related study by Keogh, Garnier, Bernheimer, and Gallimore (2000) also used a cross-lagged model that generally supported a child-driven model, specifically with regard to children’s cognitive ability and personal–social competence (i.e., daily living skills) predicting parental accommodations to the demands of daily life with a child who has delays. However, when examining behavior problems and intensity, Keogh et al. found support for a bidirectional relationship. They proposed that children with more severe behavior problems require more accommodations; however, in light of the current study, it may also be that such accommodations lead to increased parental stress, which, in turn, may exacerbate the child’s behavior problems over time. Furthermore, in a previous study ( Neece & Baker, 2008 ), we used a cross-lagged model and found that parental stress was associated with child social skills difficulties 2 years later; however, the opposite direction of effect (early child social skills predicting later parental stress) was not significant. Together, these findings highlight the importance of empirically testing directions of effect when investigating parent–child relationships.
An inherent difficulty in studies of parenting stress is that stress is a subjective construct, leading investigators to use self-report assessments that are subject to response bias. In contrast, many child behavior problems can be determined using observational measures, which may be more objective than parent reports. Given that parenting stress and child behavior problems both were assessed with parent-report questionnaires in the present study, shared method variance is a concern. One strength of the study design, however, is that measures of parenting stress and child behavior problems were administered days or weeks apart (see the Method section), which may have decreased respondent bias. In addition, the convergent findings supporting a reciprocal relationship using mother and father reports increase our confidence in these results. Furthermore, we continued to find some evidence of a transactional relationship even when the models were run using different reporters for parenting stress and child behavior problems.
Although this investigation provides a more complete analysis of the relationship between child behavior problems and parenting stress across childhood than previous studies, additional research may lead to a fuller understanding of the transactional relationship between these variables over time. Mediators of the relationship between parenting stress and child behavior problems could be examined to elucidate the pathways through which stress leads to changes in child behavior problems and vice versa. Parenting behavior may be one mediator of stress as a predictor of child behavior problems. Some research conducted with typically developing children has suggested that stress in the family context may lead to less competent and less responsive parenting ( Belsky, Woodworth, & Crnic, 1996 ; Crnic & Low, 2002; Patterson, 1983 ), which has been associated with subsequent changes in child behavior and, in extreme cases, the development of psychopathology ( Cummings et al., 2000 ; Koblinsky, Kuvalanka, & Randolph, 2006 ; Osborne, McHugh, Saunders, & Reed, 2008 ). In addition, these highly stressed parents may not model good self-regulation for their children, which may lead to more behavior problems. With regard to the opposite direction of effect (child behavior to parenting stress), child behavior problems may create more stress in the broader ecological environment (e.g., school, neighborhood), leading to augmented parental stress ( Bronfenbrenner, 1979 ).
Last, moderators of the relationship between behavior problems and parenting stress could be explored. The trajectories reported in this article represent mean changes in behavior problems and stress across development; however, it is likely that there are families for which these patterns diverge, and future research should identify moderators of changes in these variables over time. Studies should ascertain the primary risk and protective factors that change the strength of this relationship over time.
These findings have clear implications for intervention programs. The bidirectional relationship between children’s behavior problems and parenting stress highlights both variables as targets for intervention. Fortunately, there is considerable evidence that behavior problems in youth with intellectual disability can be significantly reduced through effective interventions ( Baker, 1996 ; Heyvaert, Maes, & Onghena, 2010 ; Horner et al., 2002 ; McIntyre, 2008 ). Parent-training interventions targeting child behavior problems have been found to reduce parental stress posttreatment, although these studies have been almost exclusively with cognitively typical children ( Eyberg et al., 2001 ; Feinfield & Baker, 2004 ). The long-term effects of the intervention on parenting stress may not be as strong as the long-term effects on child behavior problems ( Eyberg et al., 2001 ).
Our findings also suggest that stress management interventions may be effective in reducing parenting stress and, consequently, lead to reductions in behavior problems. Methods commonly used include progressive muscle relaxation, biofeedback, meditation, and cognitive restructuring ( Lehrer, Carr, Sargunaraj, & Woolfolk, 1994 ). Stress management techniques have been associated with decreases in symptoms of anxiety and depression ( Barlow, Rapee, Brown, 1992 ; Cruess et al., 2002 ) and better physical health outcomes ( Garcia-Vera, Sanz, & Labrador, 1998 ; Holroyd et al., 2001 ). Although the effect of these interventions on parenting practices or child behavior problems is unknown, results of this study suggest that reducing parenting stress has the potential to reduce behavior problems.
Acknowledgments
We are indebted to our staff and doctoral student colleagues.
This article was based on the activities of the Collaborative Family Study, supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Grant 34879-1459 to Bruce L. Baker, Jan Blacher, and Keith Crnic [principal investigators]).
- Abidin RR. Parenting Stress Index manual. 3. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press; 1990. [ Google Scholar ]
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist 1½–5. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington: Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, University of Vermont; 2001. [ Google Scholar ]
- Anastopoulos AD, Shelton TL, DuPaul GJ, Guevremont DC. Parent training for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Its impact on parent functioning. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1993;21:581–596. doi: 10.1007/BF00916320. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Baker BL. Parent training. In: Jacobson JW, Mulick JA, editors. Manual of diagnosis and professional practice in mental retardation. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1996. pp. 289–299. [ Google Scholar ]
- Baker BL, Blacher J, Crnic KA, Edelbrock C. Behavior problems and parenting stress in families of three-year-old children with and without developmental delays. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2002;107:433–444. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0433:BPAPSI>2.0.CO;2. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Baker BL, McIntyre LL, Blacher J, Crnic K, Edelbrock C, Low C. Pre-school children with and without developmental delay: Behaviour problems and parenting stress over time. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2003;47:217–230. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00484.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Baker BL, Neece CL, Fenning R, Crnic KA, Blacher J. Mental disorders in five year old children with or without intellectual disability: Focus on ADHD. Journal of Child Clinical and Adolescent Psychology. 2010;49:492–505. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.486321. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Barlow DH, Rapee RM, Brown TA. Behavioral treatment of generalized anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy. 1992;23:551–570. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development: Manual. 2. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1993. [ Google Scholar ]
- Belsky J. The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development. 1984;55:83–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00275.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Belsky J, Woodworth S, Crnic K. Trouble in the second year: Three questions about family interaction. Child Development. 1996b;67:556–578. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Blacher J, Baker BL, Eisenhower AS. Student–teacher relationship stability across early school years for children with intellectual disability or typical development. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2009;114:322–339. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-114.5.322. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Skuban EM, Horwitz SM. Prevalence of social-emotional and behavioral problems in a community sample of 1- and 2-year-old children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;40:811–819. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200107000-00016. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Brody GH, Ge X. Linking parenting processes and self-regulation to psychological functioning and alcohol use during early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology. 2001;15:82–94. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.15.1.82. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Bronfenbrenner U. Contexts of child rearing: Problems and prospects. American Psychologist. 1979;34:844–850. [ Google Scholar ]
- Chen TY, Tang CS. Stress appraisal and social support of Chinese mothers of adult children with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1997;101:473–482. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Cohen J, Cohen P, West S, Aiken L. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
- Coldwell J, Pike A, Dunn J. Household chaos—Links with parenting and child behaviour. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:1116–1122. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01655.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Combs-Ronto LA, Olson SL, Lunkenheimer ES, Sameroff AJ. Interactions between maternal parenting and children’s early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37:1151–1163. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Conger RD, Patterson GR, Ge X. It takes two to replicate: A mediational model for the impact of parents’ stress on adolescent adjustment. Child Development. 1995;66:80–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00857.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Connell AM, Dishion TJ. The contribution of peers to monthly variation in adolescent depressed mood: A short-term longitudinal study with time-varying predictors. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:139–154. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060081. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Cormack KFM, Brown AC, Hastings RP. Behavioural and emotional difficulties in students attending schools for children and adolescents with severe intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2000;44:124–129. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2000.00251.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Crnic KA, Gaze C, Hoffman C. Cumulative parenting stress across the pre-school period: Relations to maternal parenting and child behavior at age 5. Infant and Child Development. 2005;14:117–132. [ Google Scholar ]
- Cruess S, Antoni MH, Hayes A, Penedo F, Ironson G, Fletcher MA, … Schneiderman N. Changes in mood and depressive symptoms and related change processes during cognitive-behavioral stress management in HIV-infected men. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2002;26:373–392. [ Google Scholar ]
- Cummings EM, Davies PT, Campbell SB. Developmental psychopathology and family process: Theory, research, and clinical implications. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
- Deater-Deckard K. Parenting stress and child adjustment: Some old hypotheses and new questions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 1998;5:314–332. [ Google Scholar ]
- Deater-Deckard K, Scarr S. Parenting stress among dual-earner mothers and fathers: Are there gender differences? Journal of Family Psychology. 1996;10:45–59. [ Google Scholar ]
- Dekker MC, Koot HM. DSM-IV disorders in children with borderline to moderate intellectual disability. I: Prevalence and impact. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:915–922. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046892.27264.1A. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Dekker MC, Koot HM, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Emotional and behavioral problems in children and adolescents with and without intellectual disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2002;43:1087–1098. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00235. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- deRuiter KP, Dekker MC, Douma JCH, Verhulst FC, Koot HM. Development of parent- and teacher-reported emotional and behavioural problems in young people with intellectual disabilities: Does level of intellectual disability matter? Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities. 2008;21:70–80. [ Google Scholar ]
- Donenberg G, Baker BL. The impact of young children with externalizing behaviors on their families. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1993;21:179–198. doi: 10.1007/BF00911315. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL, Fabes RA, Shepard SA, Reiser M, … Guthrie IK. The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development. 2001;72:1112–1134. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00337. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Eisenhower AS, Baker BL, Blacher J. Children’s delayed development and behavior problems: Impact on mothers’ perceived physical health across early childhood. Social Science & Medicine. 2009;68:89–99. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.09.033. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Emerson E. Mothers of children and adolescents with intellectual disability: Social and economic situation, mental health status, and the self-assessed social and psychological impact of the child’s difficulties. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2003;47:385–399. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00498.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Emerson E, Einfeld S. Emotional and behavioural difficulties in young children with and without developmental delay: A bi-national perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:583–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02179.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Emerson E, Hatton C. Mental health of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities in Britain. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;191:493–499. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.038729. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Eyberg SM, Funderburk BW, Hembree-Kigin TL, McNeil CB, Querido JG, Hood KK. Parent-child interaction therapy with behavior problem children: One and two year maintenance of treatment effects in the family. Child & Family Behavior Therapy. 2001;23:1–20. [ Google Scholar ]
- Fanti KA, Henrich CC, Brookmeyer KA, Kuperminc GP. Toward a transactional model of parent-adolescent relationship quality and adolescent psychological adjustment. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2008;28:252–276. [ Google Scholar ]
- Feinfield KA, Baker BL. Empirical support for a treatment program for families of young children with externalizing problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33:182–195. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_17. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Floyd FJ, Singer GHS, Powers LE, Costigan CL. Families coping with mental retardation: Assessment and therapy. In: Jacobson JW, Mulick JA, editors. Manual of diagnosis and professional practice in mental retardation. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1996. pp. 277–288. [ Google Scholar ]
- García-Vera MP, Sanz J, Labrador FJ. Psychological changes accompanying and mediating stress-management training for essential hypertension. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. 1998;23:159–178. doi: 10.1023/a:1022295321208. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Glidden LM. Parents for children, children for parents: The adoption alternative. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation; 1989. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Glidden LM, Schoolcraft SA. Depression: Its trajectory and correlates in mothers rearing children with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research: Special Issue on Family Research. 2003;47:250–263. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00487.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Gottlieb G. Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science. 2007;10:1–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Greenberg JS, Seltzer MM, Hong J, Orsmond GI. Bidirectional effects of expressed emotion and behavior problems and symptoms in adolescents and adults with autism. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2006;111:229–249. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[229:BEOEEA]2.0.CO;2. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Gross HE, Shaw DS, Moilanen KL. Reciprocal associations between boys’ externalizing problems and mothers’ depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:693–709. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9224-x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hastings RP, Daley D, Burns C, Beck A. Maternal distress and expressed emotion: Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships with behavior problems of children with intellectual disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2006;111:48–61. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[48:MDAEEC]2.0.CO;2. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hauser-Cram P, Warfield ME, Shonkoff JP, Krauss MW. Children with disabilities: A longitudinal study of child development and parent well-being. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2001;66:1–131. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hay DF, Pawlby S, Sharp D, Schmücker G, Mills A, Allen H, Kumar R. Parents’ judgments about young children’s problems: Why mothers and fathers might disagree yet still predict later outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40:1249–1258. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Herring S, Gray K, Taffe J, Tonge B, Sweeney D, Einfeld S. Behaviour and emotional problems in toddlers with pervasive developmental disorders and developmental delay: Associations with parental mental health and family functioning. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2006;50:874–882. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00904.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Heyvaert M, Maes B, Onghena P. A meta-analysis of intervention effects on challenging behaviour among persons with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2010;54:634–649. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01291.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hill AL, Degnan KA, Calkins SD, Keane SP. Profiles of externalizing behavior problems for boys and girls across preschool: The roles of emotion regulation and inattention. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:913–928. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.913. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Holroyd KA, O’Donnell FJ, Stensland M, Lipchik GL, Cordingley GE, Carlson BW. Management of chronic tension-type headache with tricyclic antidepressant medication, stress management therapy, and their combination: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2001;285:2208–2215. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.17.2208. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Horner RH, Carr EG, Strain PS, Todd AW, Reed HK. Problem behavior interventions for young children with autism: A research synthesis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2002;32:423–446. doi: 10.1023/a:1020593922901. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Johnson C, Mash EJ. Families of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Review and recommendations for future research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2001;4:183–207. doi: 10.1023/a:1017592030434. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Keogh BA, Garnier HE, Bernheimer LP, Gallimore R. Models of child–family interactions for children with developmental delays: Child-driven or transactional? American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2000;105:32–46. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2000)105<0032:MOCIFC>2.0.CO;2. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Kerr DCR, Lunkenheimer ES, Olson SL. Assessment of child problem behaviors by multiple informants: A longitudinal study from preschool to school entry. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:967–975. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01776.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Kersh J, Hedvat TT, Hauser-Cram P, Warfield ME. The contribution of marital quality to the well-being of parents of children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2006;50:883–893. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00906.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Koblinsky SA, Kuvalanka KA, Randolph SM. Social skills and behavior problems of urban, African American pre-schoolers: Role of parenting practices, family conflict, and maternal depression. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2006;76:554–563. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.76.4.554. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Koskentausta T, Iivanainen M, Almqvist F. Risk factors for psychiatric disturbance in children with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51:43–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00871.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Lazarus RS. From psychological stress to the emotions: A history of changing outlooks. Annual Review of Psychology. 1993;44:1–21. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.000245. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Lehrer PM, Carr R, Sargunaraj D, Wool-folk RL. Stress management techniques: Are they all equivalent, or do they have specific effects? Biofeedback & Self Regulation. 1994;19:353–401. doi: 10.1007/BF01776735. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Mash EJ, Johnson C. Determinants of parenting stress: Illustrations from families of hyperactive children and families of physically abused children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1990;19:313–328. [ Google Scholar ]
- McCarthy J, Boyd J. Psychopathology and young people with Down’s syndrome: Childhood predictors and adult outcome of disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2001;45:99–105. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2001.00335.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- McIntyre LL. Parent training for young children with developmental disabilities: Randomized controlled trial. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;113:356–368. doi: 10.1352/2008.113:356-368. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Merrell KW, Holland ML. Social-emotional behavior of preschool-age children with and without developmental delays. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 1997;18:393–405. doi: 10.1016/s0891-4222(97)00018-8. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Neece CL, Baker BL. Predicting maternal parenting stress in middle childhood: The roles of child intellectual status, behavior problems, and social skills. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2008;52:1114–1128. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01071.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Neece CL, Blacher J, Baker BL. Impact on siblings of children with intellectual disability: The role of child behavior problems. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2010;115:291–306. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.4.291. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Oelofsen N, Richardson P. Sense of coherence and parenting stress in mothers and fathers of preschool children with developmental disability. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability. 2006;31:1–12. doi: 10.1080/13668250500349367. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Olson SL, Sameroff AJ, Lunkenheimer ES, Kerr DC. Self-regulatory processes in the development of disruptive behavior problems: The preschool-to-school transition. In: Olson SL, Sameroff AJ, editors. Biopsychosocial regulatory processes in the development of childhood behavioral problems. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2009. pp. 144–185. [ Google Scholar ]
- Orsmond GI, Seltzer MM, Krauss MW, Hong J. Behavior problems in adults with mental retardation and maternal well-being: Examination of the direction of effects. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2003;108:257–271. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108<257:BPIAWM>2.0.CO;2. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Osborne LA, McHugh L, Saunders J, Reed P. The effect of parenting behaviors on subsequent child behavior problems in autistic spectrum conditions. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2008;2:249–263. [ Google Scholar ]
- Patterson CR. Stress: A change agent for family processes. In: Garmezy N, Rutter M, editors. Stress, coping, and development in children. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1983. pp. 235–264. [ Google Scholar ]
- Pesonen A, Räikkönen K, Heinonen K, Komsi N, Järvenpää A, Strandberg T. A transactional model of temperamental development: Evidence of a relationship between child temperament and maternal stress over five years. Social Development. 2008;17:326–340. [ Google Scholar ]
- Phares V. Fathers and developmental psychopathology. Oxford, England: Wiley; 1996. [ Google Scholar ]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. 2. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
- Roggman LA, Boyce LK, Cook GA, Christiansen K, Jones D. Playing with daddy: Social toy play, early head start, and developmental outcomes. Fathering Special Issue: Fathers in Early Head Start. 2004;2:83–108. [ Google Scholar ]
- Rothbaum F, Weisz JR. Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:55–74. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.55. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Sameroff AJ. Conceptual issues in studying the development of self-regulation. In: Olson SL, Sameroff AJ, editors. Biopsychosocial regulatory processes in the development of childhood behavioral problems. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2009. pp. 1–18. [ Google Scholar ]
- Suárez LM, Baker BL. Child externalizing behavior and parents’ stress: The role of social support. Family Relations. 1997;46:373–381. [ Google Scholar ]
- Tan TX. Two-year follow-up of girls adopted from China: Continuity and change in behavioural adjustment. Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2011;16:14–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2010.00560.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Thorndike RL, Hagen E, Sattler J. The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale: Fourth edition. Chicago: Riverside; 1986. Technical manual. [ Google Scholar ]
- VanderValk I, de Goede M, Spruijt E, Meeus W. A longitudinal study on transactional relations between parental marital distress and adolescent emotional adjustment. Adolescence. 2007;42:116–136. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Wallander JL, Dekker MC, Koot HM. Psychopathology in children and adolescents with intellectual disability: Measurement, prevalence, course, and risk. In: Glidden LM, editor. International review of research in mental retardation. Vol. 26. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2003. pp. 93–134. [ Google Scholar ]
- Wills TA, Dishion TJ. Temperament and adolescent substance use: A transactional analysis of emerging self-control. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33:69–81. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_7. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Zhang X, Chen H, Zhang G, Zhou B, Wu W. A longitudinal study of parent-child relationships and problem behaviors in early childhood: Transactional model. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 2008;40:571–582. [ Google Scholar ]
- View on publisher site
- PDF (562.8 KB)
- Collections
Similar articles
Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.
- Download .nbib .nbib
- Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
IMAGES